Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T04:40:12.625Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Reactions to Pain in Various Abnormal Mental States

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2018

E. Stengel
Affiliation:
University of London, The Bethlem Royal Hospital and the Maudsley Hospital
A. J. Oldham
Affiliation:
Cane Hill Hospital, Coulsdon, Surrey, The Bethlem Royal Hospital and the Maudsley Hospital
A. S. C. Ehrenberg
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, University of London

Extract

The study on which we are going to report has its origin in a clinical observation made twenty-six years ago (Schilderand Stengel,1928).

An elderly female patient whose main symptom was a receptive aphasia, showed a peculiar reaction to painful stimuli. She failed to withdraw from the source of those stimuli, or did so only to a very slight degree, irrespective of the part of the body surface affected. She never showed that tendency to total withdrawal which characterizes normal behaviour. Sometimes she exhibited paradoxical reactions in that she followed the stimuli with her hand as if to invite more pain. There was no indication of a disturbance of perception. That the patient perceived pain could be inferred from her utterances and from wincing, which she often showed very markedly, while at the same time failing to withdraw the afflicted part of her body. This was another argument against the presence of a disturbance of sensation which did not affect the whole body surface. Within a few weeks the aphasia subsided sufficiently to enable the patient to talk about her attitude to the painful stimuli. She definitely experienced them as such but did not seem to mind them. Reactions to certain other stimuli were found to be equally abnormal. Neither a match lit close to her eyes, nor a loud clap, would cause her to withdraw in a normal manner. It did not make any noticeable difference whether these stimuli were sprung on her unexpectedly or whether she could watch them being inflicted, nor did it matter who administered them. In marked contrast to the apparent indifference to external stimuli, the patient was sensitive to internal pains and used to complain in an almost hypochondriacal manner about stomach pain. The same discrepancy was noticed in other cases. The patient's peculiar behaviour in relation to painful or other noxious stimuli inflicted from outside which are usually experienced and reacted to as threats, was assumed to be due to a disorder on a higher level of integration. It was called, perhaps not very aptly, asymbolia for pain, the patient, while able to perceive pain, being unable to appreciate its significance as a signal of danger and to react accordingly. The term is obviously not comprehensive enough and does not take into account the patient's failure to respond normally to other external stimuli to which the usual reaction is withdrawal. This behaviour pattern was subsequently observed in a considerable number of cases. Clinical observations suggested that this symptom was related to parietal lobe lesions in the dominant hemisphere, and this was borne out by a series of post-mortem examinations. Asymbolia for pain has since been observed by others and is often referred to among the effects of parietal lobe lesions. However there is still much that is obscure about it. In psychiatric hospitals one can often observe it in cases of brain atrophy in which the parietal, or parieto-temporal area is more severely involved than other parts of the brain. The symptom fits well into the present-day concepts of parietal lobe syndromes (Critchley, 1951). It implies an inability to integrate external stimuli, or, as Schilder put it, to connect the experience of pain with the body image.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1955 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bender, L., and Schilder, P., Amer. J. Psychiat., 1930, 10, 365.Google Scholar
Chapman, W. P., Psychosom. Med., 1944, 6, 252.Google Scholar
Idem , and Jones, C. M., J. Clin. Invest., 1944, 23, 81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, W. P., et al., Ibid, 1946, 25, 890.Google Scholar
Idem , Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 1947, 57, 321.Google Scholar
Idem , Res. Publ. Ass. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 1948, 27, 754.Google Scholar
Idem , Amer. J. Psychiat., 1950, 107, 221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, D., et al., Res. Publ. Ass. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 1935, 15, 417.Google Scholar
Columbia-Greystone Associates, Problems of the Human Brain, 1949. New York.Google Scholar
Critchley, M., Brit. med. J., 1934, 11, 891.Google Scholar
Idem , Brain, 1949, 72, 55.Google Scholar
Idem , Proc. Roy. Soc. Med., 1951, 44, 337.Google Scholar
Falconer, M. A., Res. Publ. Ass. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 1948, 27, 706.Google Scholar
Furer, M., and Hardy, J., Ibid, 1950, 29, 72.Google Scholar
Gellhorn, E., Physiological Foundations of Neurology and Psychiatry, 1953. K. Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Guttmann, E., and Mayer-Gross, W., Lancet, 1943, i, 225.Google Scholar
Hall, K. R. L., Brit. J. Psychol., 1953, 44, 279.Google Scholar
Hall, K. R. L., and Stride, E., Brit. J. Med. Psychol., 1954, 27, 48.Google Scholar
Hardy, J., et al., Res. Publ. Ass. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 1943, 23, 1.Google Scholar
Hemphill, R. E., and Stengel, E., J. Neurol. Psychiat., 1940, 3, 251.Google Scholar
Hemphill, R. E., et al., J. Ment. Sci., 1952, 98, 433.Google Scholar
Hines, F. A., and Brown, G. E., Proc. Mayo Clinic, 1932, 7, 332.Google Scholar
Hoskins, R. G., The Biology of Schizophrenia, 1946. New York.Google Scholar
Igersheimer, W. W., Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 1953, 70, 794.Google Scholar
Kennard, M. A., J. Clin. Invest., 1952, 31, 245.Google Scholar
Lewis, N. D. C., Nerv. Ment. Dis. Mon., 1923, 35.Google Scholar
Malmo, R. B., et al., Sci., 1948, 108, 509.Google Scholar
Malmo, R. B., and Shagass, C., Psychosom. Med., 1949a, 11, 9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Idem , Ibid., 1949b, 11, 25.Google Scholar
Malmo, R. B., et al., J. Personality, 1951a, 19, 359.Google Scholar
Idem , J. Clin. Psychopath., 1951b, 12, 45.Google Scholar
Medvei, V. C., The Mental and Physical Effects of Pain, 1949. Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Oldham, A. J., J. Ment. Sci., 1953, 99, 580.Google Scholar
Popper, E., Neurol. Centralblatt, 1920, 1, 1.Google Scholar
Rubins, J. L., and Friedman, E. D., Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 1948, 60, 554.Google Scholar
Shattock, F. M., J. Ment. Sci., 1950, 96, 32.Google Scholar
Schilder, P., and Stengel, E., Zeitschr. ges. Neurol. Psychiat., 1928a, 113, 143.Google Scholar
Idem , Klin. Wochenschr., 1928b, 12, 535.Google Scholar
Idem , Zeitschr. ges. Neurol. Psychiat., 1931a, 132, 143.Google Scholar
Idem , Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 1931b, 25, 598.Google Scholar
Schilling, R. F., and Musser, M. J., Amer. J. Med. Sci., 1948, 215, 195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stengel, E., J. Ment. Sci., 1943, 89, 1.Google Scholar
Idem , Ibid., 1944, 90, 753.Google Scholar
Idem , Ibid., 1947, 93, 589.Google Scholar
Idem , Ibid., 1948, 94, 46.Google Scholar
Watts, J. W., and Freeman, W., Res. Publ. Ass. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 1948, 27, 715.Google Scholar
Wolf, S., and Hardy, J. D., Ibid., 1943, 23, 123.Google Scholar
Wolff, H. H., Quart. J. Med., 1951, 20, 261.Google Scholar
Zilboorg, , Res. Publ. Ass. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 1948, 27, 706.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.