Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T08:58:08.668Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Night Vision and Psychiatric Disorders: A Review of Experimental Studies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2018

G. W. Granger*
Affiliation:
Psychological Department, Institute of Psychiatry, Maudsley Hospital, S.E.5

Extract

In recent years a number of studies have been undertaken in which the “night vision” or dark-adaptation of psychiatric patients has been compared with that of normal subjects. These studies have their origin in a wartime observation that the incidence of “night-blindness” among neurotics was higher than among normal Service personnel. Evidence of functional disorders of vision is of interest from several points of view, psychiatric and ophthalmological as well as psychological and physiological. The aim of this article is to make a critical evaluation of the results so far obtained, determine what generalizations are possible and consider implications for further research.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1957 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

1. Adrian, E. D., “The nervous reactions of the retina”, Trans. Illumin. Eng. Soc., 1953, 18, 105112.Google Scholar
2. Idem and Matthews, R., “The action of light on the eye. Pt. III. The interaction of retinal neurones”, J. Physiol., 1928, 65, 273298.Google Scholar
2a. Altschule, M. D., Bodily physiology in mental and emotional disorders, 1953. New York: Grune and Stratton.Google Scholar
3. Arden, G. B., and Weale, R. A., “Nervous mechanisms and dark adaptation”, J. Physiol., 1954, 125, 417426.Google Scholar
4. Babskii, E. B., “The significance of the sympathetic nervous system in regulation of excitability of the visual analyser”, Probl. fiziol. Optiki, 1947, 4, 1730.Google Scholar
5. Idem and Skulov, D. R., “The effect of sympathectomy on visual adaptation to darkness”, Biull. eksper. Biol. i Medits., 1944, 18 (7–8), 5961.Google Scholar
6. Bartley, S. H., Vision, 1941. New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
7. Baumgardt, E., Les théories photochimiques classiques et quantiques de la vision et l'inhibition nerveuse en vision liminaire, 1950. Paris: Editions de la Revue d'Optique.Google Scholar
8. Berry, W., Review of wartime studies of dark adaptation, night vision tests, and related topics, 1949. Vision Committee Secretariat, Univ. Michigan, Ann Arbor.Google Scholar
9. Birren, J. E., Casperson, R. C., and Botwinick, J., “Age changes in pupil size”, J. Gerontol., 1950, 5, 216221.Google Scholar
10. Bitterman, M. E., Krauskopf, J., and Hochberg, J. E., “Threshold for visual form: a diffusion model”, Amer. J. Psychol., 1954, 67, 205219.Google Scholar
11. Blackwell, H. R., “Studies of psychophysical methods for measuring visual thresholds”, J. opt. Soc. Amer., 1952, 42, 606616.Google Scholar
12. Blake, R. R., and Ramsey, G. V., (Eds.), Perception: an approach to personality, 1951. New York: Ronald Press.Google Scholar
13. Borel, G., “Affections hystériques des muscles oculaires”, Arch. d'Ophthal., 1886, 6, 481536.Google Scholar
14. Bouman, M. A., and Ten Doesschate, J., “Nervous and photochemical components in visual adaptation”, Ophthalmologica, 1953, 126, 222230.Google Scholar
15. Brown, M. A., and Marshall, A. J., Personal communication.Google Scholar
16. Campbell, F. W., “The minimum quantity of light required to elicit the accommodation reflex in man”, J. Physiol., 1954, 123, 357366.Google Scholar
17. Chapanis, A., and Rouse, R. O., A German dark adaptometer. AAF Air Technical Service Command Eng. Div. Memo. Rep. Ser. No. TSEAL3-695-48K, 1945.Google Scholar
18. Charcot, J., and Galezowsky, X., “Contracture hystérique de l'iris et du muscle accommodateur avec myopie consécutive”, Prog. Med., 1878, 6, 3940.Google Scholar
19. Craik, K. J. W., and Vernon, M. D., “The nature of dark adaptation”, Brit. J. Psychol., 1941–42, 32, 6281.Google Scholar
20. Idem , “Perception during dark adaptation”, ibid, 1942, 32, 206230.Google Scholar
21. Crawford, B. H., “Dependence of pupil size upon external light stimulus under static and variable conditions”, Proc. roy. Soc., 1937, 121B, 376395. London.Google Scholar
22. Idem , “Ocular interaction in its relation to measurements of brightness threshold”, Proc. roy. Soc. B, 1940, 128, 552559.Google Scholar
23. DeJong, R. N., The neurologic examination, 1950. London: Cassell.Google Scholar
24. Ditchburn, R. W., and Power-Steele, E. J., “Early stages of dark-adaptation in the central parts of the retina”, Proc. roy. Irish Acad., 1942, 48A, 5589.Google Scholar
25. Duke-Elder, W. S., Textbook of ophthalmology, 1938. London: Kimpton.Google Scholar
26. Elsberg, C. A., and Spotnitz, H., “The neural components of light and dark adaptation and their significance for the duration of the foveal dark adaptation process”, Bull. Neurol. Inst. N. Y., 1938, 7, 148159.Google Scholar
27. Eysenck, H. J., “A dynamic theory of anxiety and hysteria”, J. Ment. Sci., 1955, 101, 2851.Google Scholar
28. Idem , “Reminiscence, drive and personality theory”, J. abnorm. soc. Psychol. (to appear).Google Scholar
29. Idem , Granger, G. W., and Brengelmann, J. C., Perceptual functions and mental illness, Maudsley Monogr. Ser., 1957. London: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar
30. Franks, C. M., “An experimental study of conditioning as related to mental abnormality”, 1954. Ph.D. Thesis, Univ. Lond. Google Scholar
31. Frenkel-Brunswick, E., “Intolerance of ambiguity as an emotional and perceptual personality variable”, J. Personality, 1949, 18, 108143.Google Scholar
32. Gellhorn, E., “The effect of oxygen lack, variations in CO2 content of inspired air, and hyperpnea on visual intensity discrimination”, Amer. J. Physiol., 1936, 115, 679684.Google Scholar
33. Idem , Feldman, J., and Allen, A., “Effect of emotional excitement on the insulin content of the blood”, Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 1942, 47, 234244.Google Scholar
34. Godding, E. W., “The testing of night vision”, Trans. Illum. Eng. Soc., 1945, 10 (2), 2741.Google Scholar
35. Goodman, L. S., and Gilman, A., The pharmacological basis of therapeutics, 1955. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
36. Granger, G. W., “Personality and visual perception: a review”, J. Ment. Sci., 1953, 99, 843.Google Scholar
37. Idem , “Effect of ephedrine on dark vision”, Nature, 1954, 174, 653.Google Scholar
38. Idem , “The night visual ability of psychiatric patients”, Brit. J. physiol. Opt., 1954, 11, 226232.Google Scholar
39. Idem , “Dark adaptation in neurotic patients”, J. Ment. Sci., 1955, 101, 354362.Google Scholar
40. Idem , “Dark adaptation time in neurotic patients”, Brit. J. physiol. Opt., 1956, 13, 3944.Google Scholar
41. Idem , “Dark adaptation in anxiety states and hysterics”, ibid, 1956, 13, 234.Google Scholar
42. Granit, R., Sensory mechanisms of the retina, 1947. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
43. Idem , “The physiological significance of the retinal synapses. Report of a joint discussion on vision”, The Physical and Optical Societies, 1932, pp. 263271.Google Scholar
44. Idem , Munsterhjelm, A., and Zewi, M., “The relation between concentration of visual purple and retinal sensitivity to light during dark adaptation”, J. Physiol., 1939, 96, 3144.Google Scholar
45. Gravely, A. M., Ph.D. Thesis, 1950, Univ. London.Google Scholar
46. Haig, C., “The course of dark adaptation as influenced by the intensity and duration of pre-adaptation to light”, J. gen. Physiol., 1941, 24, 735.Google Scholar
47. Hammond, E. C., and Lee, R. H., “A method of scoring dark-adaptation tests”, U.S. Nat. Inst. Health, 1941, 30 June.Google Scholar
48. Idem , “Criteria for scoring dark-adaptation tests”, U.S. Nat. Inst. Health Div. Indust-Hygiene, 1941, 10 July.Google Scholar
49. Hecht, S., and Mandelbaum, J., “The relation between vitamin A and dark adaptation”, J. Amer. med. Ass., 1939, 112, 19101916.Google Scholar
50. Himmelweit, H. T., Desaie, M., and Petrie, A., “An experimental investigation of neuroticism”, J. Personality, 1946, 15, 173196.Google Scholar
51. Holzer, W., “Unpublished experiments referred to in Rose, H. W., and Schmidt, I. Factors affecting dark adaptation”, J. aviat. Med., 1947, 18, 218230.Google Scholar
52. Honda, H., “Effect of the stimulus area of previous light adaptation upon the course of dark adaptation”, Acta Soc. Ophthal. Jap., 1952, 56, 519524.Google Scholar
53. Hull, C. L., Principles of behaviour, 1949. New York: Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
54. Hunt, E., and Palmer, C. E., “Medical evaluation of nutritional status. II. Measurements of visual dark adaptation with the adaptometer”, Millbank Mem. Fund Quart. J., 1940, 18, 403424.Google Scholar
55. Ives, W. C., and Shilling, C. W., “Object identification with the Hecht-Shlaer adaptometer”, Night Vision Board, U.S. Sub. Base, New London, S24-1 (102).Google Scholar
56. Janda, E., Ph.D. Thesis, Univ. Michigan, 1951.Google Scholar
57. Johnson, E. P., “The electrical response of the human retina during dark adaptation”, J. exp. Psychol., 1949, 39, 597609.Google Scholar
58. Idem and Riggs, L. A., “Electroretinal and psychophysical dark-adaptation curves”, J. exp. Psychol., 1951, 41, 139147.Google Scholar
59. Karpe, G., and Tansley, K., “The relationship between the change in the electro-retinogram and the subjective dark-adaptation curve”, J. Physiol., 1948, 107, 272279.Google Scholar
60. Klein, G. S., “The personal world through perception”. Chapter XII in Blake, R. R., and Ramsey, G. V. (Eds.), Perception: an approach to personality, 1951. New York: Ronald Press.Google Scholar
61. Krauskopf, J., Duryea, R. A., and Bitterman, M. E., “Threshold for visual form: further experiments”, Amer. J. Psychol., 1954, 67, 427440.Google Scholar
62. Krieger, H. P., and Bender, M. B., “Dark adaptation in perimetrically blind fields”, Arch. Ophthal., 1951, 46, 625636.Google Scholar
63. Kuffler, S. W., “Neurones in the retina: organization, inhibition and excitation problems”, Cold. Spr. Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol., 1952, 17, 281292.Google Scholar
64. Idem , “Discharge patterns and functional organization of mammalian retina”, J. Neurophysiol., 1953, 16, 3768.Google Scholar
65. Landis, C., “Crozier and Wolf on flicker-fusion, 1933–1944”, J. Psychol., 1954, 37, 317.Google Scholar
66. Lee, R. H., Finch, E. M., and Pounds, G. A., “Periodic fluctuations in the dark-adaptation threshold”, Amer. J. Physiol., 1945, 143, 610.Google Scholar
67. Livingston, P. C., and Bolton, B., “Night visual capacity in psychological cases”, Lancet, 1943, i, 263.Google Scholar
68. Low, F. N., “Effect of training on acuity of peripheral vision”, Civil Aeronautics Administration, Div. of Research, Report No. 68, Washington, D.C., 1946.Google Scholar
69. Idem , “Peripheral visual acuity”, Arch. Ophthal., 1951, 45, 8099.Google Scholar
70. Lythgoe, R. J., “The mechanism of dark adaptation, a critical résumé”, Brit. J. Ophthal., 1940, 24, 2143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
71. Lythgoe, R. R., and Phillips, L. R., “Binocular summation during dark adaptation”, J. Physiol., 1937, 91, 427436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
72. Lythgoe, R. J., and Tansley, K., “Adaptation of the eye: its relation to the critical flicker frequency”, M.R.C. Spec. Rep. Ser. No. 134, H.M.S.O., London, 1929.Google Scholar
73. McFarland, R. A., “Anoxia: its effects on the physiology and biochemistry of the brain and behaviour”, in Biology of mental health and disease, 1952. London: Cassell.Google Scholar
74. McFarland, R. A., and Barach, A. L., “The response of psychoneurotics to variations in oxygen tension”, Amer. J. Psychiat., 1937, 93, 13151341.Google Scholar
75. McFarland, R. A., and Evans, J. N., “Alterations of dark adaptation under reduced oxygen tensions”, Amer. J. Physiol., 1939, 127, 3750.Google Scholar
76. McFarland, R. A., and Fisher, M. B., “Alterations in dark adaptation as a function of age”, J. Gerontol., 1955, 10, 424428.Google Scholar
76a. McFarland, R. A., Halperin, M. H., and Niven, J. I., “Visual thresholds as an index of physiological imbalance during anoxia”, Amer. J. Physiol., 1944, 142, 328349.Google Scholar
77. Idem , “Visual thresholds as an index of physiological imbalance during insulin hypoglycemia”, ibid, 1946, 145, 299313.Google Scholar
78. Mandelbaum, J., “Dark adaptation: some physiologic and clinical considerations”, Arch. Ophthal., 1941, 26, 203239.Google Scholar
79. Marazzi, A. S., “The central inhibitory action of adrenaline and related compounds”, Fed. Proc., 1943, 2, 33.Google Scholar
80. Idem , “Some indicators of cerebral humoral mechanisms”, Science, 1953, 118, 367370.Google Scholar
81. Marshall, A. J., and Day, R., Personal communication.Google Scholar
82. Miles, W. R., “Light sensitivity and form perception in dark adaptation”, J. opt. Soc. Amer., 1953, 43, 560566.Google Scholar
83. Morax, V., Encyclo. franc. d'Ophtal, 1905, 4, 546.Google Scholar
84. Mote, F. A., Riopelle, A. J., and Meyer, D. R., “The effect of intermittent pre-adapting light upon subsequent dark adaptation in the human eye”, J. opt. Soc. Amer., 1950, 40, 584588.Google Scholar
85. Mote, F. A., and Riopelle, A. J., “The effect of varying the intensity and the duration of pre-exposure upon subsequent dark-adaptation in the human eye”, J. comp. physiol. Psychol., 1953, 46, 4955.Google Scholar
86. Orbeli, L. A., “Higher nervous activity and the adaptive-trophic role of the sympathetic nervous system and of the cerebellum”, Fiziol Zh. SSSR, 1949, 35(5), 594595.Google Scholar
87. Phillips, L. R., “Some factors producing individual differences in dark adaptation”, Proc. roy. Soc. B, 1939, 127, 405424.Google Scholar
88. Pirenne, M. H., “Binocular and uniocular thresholds of vision”, Nature, 1943, 152, 698699.Google Scholar
89. Idem , “The absolute sensitivity of the eye and the variation of visual acuity with intensity”, Brit. med. Bull, 1953, 9, 6167.Google Scholar
90. Idem and Denton, E. J., “Acuity and sensitivity of the human eye”, Nature, 1952, 170, 1039.Google Scholar
91. PIRENNE, M. H., Marriott, F. H. C., and O'Doherty, E. F., Individual differences in night vision efficiency. MRC Spec. Rep. Ser., 1957. London: H.M.S.O. Google Scholar
92. Plantegna, H. G. W., “Accommodatiekramp”, Ned. tij. Gen., 1908, 1, 795800.Google Scholar
93. Rees, W. L. L., “Night visual capacity of neurotic soldiers”, J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat., 1945, 8, 3439.Google Scholar
94. Riggs, L. A., “Electroretinography in cases of night blindness”, Amer. J. Ophthal., 1954, 38, 7078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
95. Idem , Armington, J. C., and Ratcuff, F., “Motions of the retinal image during fixation”, J. opt. Soc. Amer., 1954, 44, 315321.Google Scholar
96. Robertson, G. W., and Yudkin, J., “Effect of age upon dark adaptation”, J. Physiol., 1944, 103, 18.Google Scholar
97. Rose, H. W., and Schmidt, I., “Factors affecting dark adaptation”, J. aviat. Med., 1947, 18, 218230.Google Scholar
97a. Rothan, H., “Über die Beeinflussung der Netzhautfunktion durch Adrenalin”, Klin. Monatsbl.f. Augenh., 1925, 75, 747.Google Scholar
98. Rowland, W. M., and Rowland, L. S., Aspects of night visual efficiency, 1943. U.S. AAF. Sch. Aviat. Med., Randolph Field, Texas, 1 May.Google Scholar
99. Rushton, W. A. H., “Measurement of rhodopsin in the living eye”, Acta physiol., Scand., 1953, 29, 1630.Google Scholar
100. Idem , “Chemical and nervous factors in dark adaptation”, Trans. Ophthal. Soc. U.K., 1952, 72, 657664.Google Scholar
101. Idem and Campbell, F. W., “Measurement of rhodopsin in the living human eye”, Nature, 1954, 174, 1096.Google Scholar
102. Rushton, W. A. H., and Cohen, R. D., “Visual purple level and the course of dark adaptation”, Nature, 1954, 173, 301302.Google Scholar
103. Schouten, J. F., and Ornstein, L. S., “Measurements on direct and indirect adaptation by means of a binocular method”, J. opt. Soc. Amer., 1939, 29, 168182.Google Scholar
104. Schumann, W. P., “The objective determination of visual acuity on the basis of the optokinetic nystagmus”, Amer. J. Optom., 1952, 29, 575583.Google Scholar
105. Schweitzer, A., and Wright, S., “The action of adrenalin on the knee jerk”, J. Physiol., 1937, 88, 476491.Google Scholar
106. Segal, P., “Badanie adaptacji narzadu wzroku do ciemnosci”, 1953. Warsaw: PZWL.Google Scholar
107. Shagass, C., “The sedation threshold: a method for estimating tension in psychiatric patients”, EEG clin. Neurophysiol., 1954, 6, 221233.Google Scholar
108. Idem and Naiman, J., “The sedation threshold as an objective index of manifest anxiety in psychoneurosis”, J. Psychosom. Res., 1956, 1, 4957.Google Scholar
109. Shastid, T. H., “Hysteropia, or the ocular manifestations of hysteria”, Amer. J. Physiol. Opt., 1921, 2, 289292.Google Scholar
110. Sheard, C., “Dark adaptation: some physical, physiological, clinical and aeromedical considerations”, J. opt. Soc., Amer., 1944, 34, 464508.Google Scholar
110a. Simonson, E., and Brozek, J., “Flicker fusion frequency: background and applications”, Physiological Reviews, 1952, 32, 349378.Google Scholar
111. Sloan, L. L., “Size of pupil as a variable factor in the determination of the light minimum”, Arch. Ophthal., 1940, 24, 258275.Google Scholar
112. Stavraky, G. W., “The action of adrenalin on spinal neurones sensitized by partial isolation”, Amer. J. Physiol., 1947, 150, 3745.Google Scholar
113. Steadman, B. St. J., “An investigation of night vision among personnel of an A.A. unit”, J. roy. Army med. Corps, 1942, 78, 1424.Google Scholar
114. Steven, D. M., “Relation between dark adaptation and age”, Nature, 1946, 157, 376377.Google Scholar
115. Tansley, K., “The regeneration of visual purple: its relation to dark adaptation and night blindness”, J. Physiol., 1931, 71, 442458.Google Scholar
116. Therman, P. O., “The neurophysiology of the retina in the light of chemical methods of modifying its excitability”, Acta Soc. Sci. Fenn., 1938, NSB, II, 1.Google Scholar
117. Thomson, L. C., “The influence of variations in the light history of the eye upon the course of its dark adaptation”, J. Physiol., 1949, 109, 430438.Google Scholar
118. Trent, S. E., “Enhancement of border contrast by pentobarbital”, J. gen. Psychol., 1947, 36, 6578.Google Scholar
119. Van Den Brink, G., and Bouman, M. A., “Variation of integrative actions in the retinal system: an adaptation phenomenon”, J. opt. Soc. Amer., 1954, 44, 616620.Google Scholar
120. Wald, G., Harper, P. V., Goodman, H. C., and Krieger, H. P., “Respiratory effects upon the visual threshold”, J. gen. Physiol., 1942, 25, 891903.Google Scholar
121. Wolf, E., and Zigler, M. J., “Dark adaptation level and size of test field”, J. opt. Soc. Amer., 1950,40, 211218.Google Scholar
122. Wright, W. D., “Night vision”, in Modern Trends in Ophthalmology. Sorsby, A. (Ed.), 1948. London: Butterworth.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.