Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-9pm4c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T13:31:02.028Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Some psychological aspects of intra-muscular injection

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2018

L. R. C. Haward*
Affiliation:
Graylingwell Hospital, Chichester Sussex

Extract

Taking the needle and being needled are contemporary colloquialisms which have some significance to those who administer injections for psychological purposes. They suggest that the experience of receiving hypodermic injections must be a relatively common one in our society, and also that such an experience is generally considered to be unpleasant. The corollary of these conclusions is that such a public attitude of mind is likely to foster preconceptions in relations to injections so that, whether or not the pain threshold itself is actually exceeded, the individual receiving an injection will be predisposed to regard the experience as a stressful one.

Type
Psychological
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1962 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bilisony, F. N., et al. (1954). “Vasodilation and lowered pain threshold”, Arch. Int. Med., 94, 759.Google Scholar
Birren, J. E., et al. (1951). “Pain measurement by the radiant heat method”, J. Exp. Psychol., 41, 419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bishop, G. H. (1943). “Responses to electrical stimulation of single sensory units of skin”, J. Neurophysiol., 6, 361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Idem (1944). “Peripheral units of pain”, J. Neurophysiol., 7, 71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buytendijk, F. J. J. (1961). Pain. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Clausen, J., et al. (1957). “Changes in G.S.R. as indication of pain threshold”, J. Genet. Psychol., 49, 261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drever, J. (1957). “A note on the classification of stimuli”, Riv. di Psicol., 50, 105.Google Scholar
Idem (1961). “Perception and action”, Bull. Brit. Psychol. Soc., 43, 1.Google Scholar
Edwards, W. (1950). “Recent research on pain perception”, Psychol. Bull., 47, 449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Euthorn, A., et al. (1955). Prefrontal Leucotomy and the Anticipation of Pain.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1960). Handbook of Abnormal Psychology. London: Pitman.Google Scholar
Gilliatt, R. W. and Pratt, R. T. C. (1952). “Disorders of perception and performance in a case of rightsided cerebral thrombosis”, J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat., 15, 264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregg, E. C. (1951). “Physical basis of pain threshold. Measurement in man”, J. Appl. Physiol., 4, 351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, K. R. L. (1953). Studies in cutaneous pain, Quart. J. Exp. Psychol., 44, 279.Google ScholarPubMed
Idem (1955). “Relation of skin temperature to pain threshold”, Quart. J. Exp. Psychol., 7, 74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hambling, J. (1959). In The Nature of Stress Disorder. London, Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Hardy, J. D., et al. (1940). “Studies in pain”, J. Clin. Invest., 19, 649.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Idem (1943). “Pain threshold in man”, Res. Pub. Ass. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 23, 1.Google Scholar
Idem (1947). “Pain threshold”, J. Clin. Invest., 26, 1152.Google Scholar
Hardy, J. D. (1953). Threshold of Pain and Reflex Contraction as related to Noxious Stimulation.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Idem (1953). “Hospitalisation stress: psychological reactions of tuberculous inpatients”, Med. World, 79, 126.Google Scholar
Idem (1955a). “The Palmer injector: a preliminary report”, Med. World, 82, 583.Google Scholar
Idem (1955b). “The mechanical injector”, Lancet, ii, 144.Google Scholar
Idem (1955c). “The mechanical injector: a new medical instrument”, Chem. Drugg., 174, 4762.Google Scholar
Idem (1957). “The Palmer injector in midwifery”, Mid. Chron., 70, 300.Google Scholar
Haward, L. R. C. (1960). “The subjective meaning of stress”, Brit. J. Med. Psychol., 33, 185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Idem (1961). “Subjective variables in physiological recording”, Acta Biotheoretica (in press).Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1954). In: Brain Mechanisms ' Consciousness. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Helson, H. and Quantius, L. (1934). “Changes in skin temperature following intense stimulation”, J. Exp. Psychol., 17, 20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemphill, R. E., et al. (1952). “Pain thresholds in psychiatric patients”, J. Ment. Sci., 98, 433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jennison, R. F. and Ellis, H. R. (1954). “Intramuscular iron a clinical trial in pregnancy”, Lancet, ii, 1245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mittelman, B. and Wolff, H. G. (1939). “Affective states and skin temperature”, Psychosom. Med., 1,271.Google Scholar
Piercy, M., et al. (1955). “Anxiety and the autonomic reaction to pain”, J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat., 18, 155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudolf, G. de M. (1955). “Clinical bloodpressure and anxiety”, J. Ment. Sci., 101, 893.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schachter, J. (1957). “Pain, fear and anger in hypertensives and normals”, Psychosom. Med., 19, 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, R. A. (1955). “Acute effects of reserpine and of sodium amytal on central sympathetic activity”, Annals. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 61, 150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stengel, E., et al. (1955). “Reactions to pain in various abnormal mental states”, J. Ment. Sci., 101, 52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trites, D. K. (1957). “Graphic determination of significance of 2 ⋉ 2 contingency tables”, Psychol. Bull., 54, 141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wenger, M. A., et al. (1956). “Physiological psychology”, Constable,—p. 102.Google Scholar
Wolff, H. G. and Wolf, S. (1948). Pain. Springfield, Thomas.Google ScholarPubMed
Zuckerman, M. and Gross, H. J. (1959). “The Funkenstein test”, J. Abnorm. ' Soc. Psychol., 59, 145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.