Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-p566r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T16:01:59.726Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Melancholia and Barbiturates: A Controlled Eeg, Body and Eye Movement Study of Sleep

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 January 2018

Ian Oswald
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Edinburgh
Ralph J. Berger
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Edinburgh
R. A. Jaramillo
Affiliation:
Su ‘Isidro Psychiatric Hospital, Cali, Colombia
K. M. G. Keddie
Affiliation:
Barrow and Glenside Hospitals, Bristol
P. C. Olley
Affiliation:
Professorial Unit, Royal Edinburgh Hospital
G. B. Plunkett
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Health, Queen's University, Belfast

Extract

Insomnia is widely accepted as a leading feature of that illness designated by the number 301.1 in the International Classification of Diseases. The name given varies—“endogenous depression”, “manic-depressive psychosis/depressive type”, “melancholia” or “depressive illness”. The present generation of laymen (not to say some psychiatrists) use the word “depression” so freely in order to describe an unpleasant, unhappy mood, whether lasting or transient, that one may prefer the term melancholia, or at least “depressive illness”, to signify that condition which justifies the number 301.1. The adjective “endogenous” implies for many that environmental stress plays little or no part in the onset of the illness; we cannot accept that this is always so. The term “reactive” is used in several ways, generally to indicate that the state of unhappiness arises out of some external circumstance and that it would end when circumstances improved or the individual accepted the realities of practical life. The patients used in the present study were suffering from an illness of a kind which we believe may sometimes develop in the absence of severe environmental stress, while in others it may be clearly provoked by circumstances, but the illness, as it develops, may take on a form which becomes largely independent of the environmental circumstances and may continue even when the provoking factors are past; it has become an autonomous melancholia. It will be apparent that by autonomous melancholia we mean an illness shown by clinical experience to respond especially well to electroplexy.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1963 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aserinsky, E., and Kleitman, N. (1955). J. appl. Physiol., 8, 1.Google Scholar
Benoit, O., and Bloch, V. (1960). J. Physiol. (Paris), 52, 17.Google Scholar
Berger, R.J. (1961). Science, 134, 840.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, R.J. and Oswald, I. (1962). J. ment. Sci., 108 (in press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, R.J. Olley, P., and Oswald, I. (1962). Quart. J. exp. Psychol., 14 (in press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brazier, M. A. B., and Beecher, H. K. (1952). J. appl. Physiol., 4, 819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, C. McC., Hoffman, B. F., Suckling, E. E., Kleyntjens, F., Koenig, E. H., Coleman, K. S., and Trueman, H. J. (1956). J. appl. Physiol., 9, 97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coleman, P. D., Gray, F. E., and Watanabe, K. (1959). J. appl. Physiol., 14, 397.Google Scholar
Dement, W. (1960). Scíence, 131, 1705.Google Scholar
Dement, W. and Kleitman, N. (1957a). Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol., 9, 673.Google Scholar
Dement, W. and Kleitman, N. (1957b). J. exp. Psychol., 53, 339.Google Scholar
Dement, W. and Wolpert, E. A. (1958). J. exp. Psychol., 55, 543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diaz-Guerrero, R., Gottlieb, J. S., and Knott, J. R. (1946). Psychosom. Med., 8, 399.Google Scholar
Goodenough, D. R., Shapiro, A., Holden, M., and Steinschreiber, L. (1959). J. abnorm. soc. Psychol., 59, 295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Held, R., Schwartz, B. A., and Fischgold, H. (1959). Presse mid., 67, 141.Google Scholar
Hinton, J. M., and Marley, E. (1959). J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiat., 22, 137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jouvet, M., Michel, F., and Mounier, D. (1960). Rev. neurol., 103, 189.Google Scholar
Kleitman, N. (1939). “Sleep and Wakefulness as Alternating Phases in the Cycle of Existence”. Chicago.Google Scholar
Loomis, A. L., Harvey, E. N., and Hobart, G. A. (1937). J. exp. Psychol., 21, 127.Google Scholar
McGhie, A., and Russell, S. M. (1962). J. ment. Sci., 108, 642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oswald, I. (1960). Brit. med. J., 1, 1450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oswald, I. (1962a). “Sleeping and Waking: Physiology and Psychology”. Amsterdam.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oswald, I. (1962b). Proc. roy. Soc. Med., 56 (in press).Google Scholar
Rossi, G. F., Favale, E., Giussani, A., and Candia, O. (1961). Fifth Int. Congr. Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol. , Excerpt a Med., Amst., p. 60.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. A., and Fischgold, H. (1960). Vie méd., 41, 39.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.