Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T21:44:19.540Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Regression from a Biological Standpoint

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 January 2018

Thomas D. Power*
Affiliation:
Spelga, Killowen, Co. Down, Northern Ireland

Extract

The term regression as used in psychiatry may be defined as the re-living by an individual of his past, or part of it, in the present. Thus, in psychoanalytic therapy the patient may treat the analyst as he did one of his parents in his childhood, while in the dream of the normal person primitive tendencies and mechanisms of thought are revived. It should be noted, however, that regression is rarely or ever clear-cut or total. Although the patient reacts towards the psychoanalyst as he did in his childhood towards his father, yet at the same time he remains well aware of the analyst's true identity. Again, in the dream, although the underlying thoughts and feelings are related to childhood they are disguised and distorted by being mixed up with experiences of much later origin. In both the transference situation and the dream there is a ‘telescoping’ of the past into the present, a fusing of chronological levels, and a ‘condensation’ of experience.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1971 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barcroft, J. (1938). The Brain and its Environment. New Haven: Yale University Press, p. 33.Google Scholar
Barcroft, J. (1941). ‘Evolution of function in the mammalian organism.’ Nature, London, 147, 762–71.Google Scholar
Goghill, G. E. (1929). Anatomy and the Problem of Behaviour, London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goghill, G. E. (1943). Journal of Comparative Neurology. 79, 463–86.Google Scholar
Sirde Beer, Gavin (1954). Embryos and Ancestors. Oxford: Clarendon Press, p. 124.Google Scholar
Dunbar, H. F. (1954). Emotion and Bodily Change. New York: Columbia University Press, p. 35.Google Scholar
Freeman, W. (1929). ‘Reversion to primitive behaviour resulting from organic disease of the brain.Human Biology, 1, 406.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1922). Beyond the Pleasure Principle. London: International Psychoanalytic Press, p. 18.Google Scholar
Grey Walter, W. (1957). The Living Brain. London: Duckworth and Co., p. 61.Google Scholar
Hardy, A. C. (1954). in Evolution as a Process. (Ed. Huxley, J.). London: Allen and Unwin, p. 123.Google Scholar
Harper, R. M. J. (1962). Evolution and Illness. Edinburgh and London: E. and S. Livingstone, p. 20.Google Scholar
Huxley, J. (1954). Evolution as a Process. London: Allen and Unwin, p. 20.Google Scholar
Jones, E. (1948). Papers on Psychoanalysis. London: Baillière, Tindall and Cox, p. 50.Google Scholar
Martin, J. P. (1937). British Medical Journal, November, p. 1062.Google Scholar
Meerloo, J. A. M. (1962). ‘The dual meaning of human regression.’ Psychoanalysis and the Psychoanalytic Review, 49, 77–36.Google Scholar
Morgan, C. T. (1943). Physiological Psychology. New York: McGraw Hill Book Co., p. 348.Google Scholar
Needham, A. E. (1962). ‘Is there a second childhood?The Gerontologist, 2, 913.Google Scholar
Needham, A. E. (1965). ‘Regeneration in the arthropoda and its endocrine control.’ In Regeneration in Animals and Related Problems. (Ed. Kiortis, V. and Trampusch, A. L.) Amsterdam: North Holland Publishing Co., 283323.Google Scholar
Paterson, A., and Zanowill, O. L. (1944). ‘Recovery of spatial orientation in the post-traumatic confusional state.Brain, 67, 54.Google Scholar
Penfield, W. (1958). ‘The role of the temporal cortex in recall of past experience and interpretation of the present.’ in Neurological Basis of Behaviour. (Ed. Wolstenholme and O'Connor.) p. 148.Google Scholar
Penfield, W. and Jasper, H. (1954). Epilepsy and the Functional Anatomy of the Human Brain, London: J. and A. Churchill, p. 64.Google Scholar
Penfield, W. and Rasmussen, T. (1950). The Cerebral Cortex of Man. New York: Macmillan and Co., p. 89.Google Scholar
Power, T. D. (1945). ‘Psychosomatic regression in therapeutic epilepsy.Psychosomatic Medicine, 7, 279–90.Google Scholar
Power, T. D. (1950). ‘The problem of the therapeutic convulsion.British Medical Journal, ii, 1092.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Power, T. D. (1955). ‘Reversal of evolution in insulin coma.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 121, 535–44.Google Scholar
Power, T. D. (1957). ‘A psychiatrist looks at epilepsy.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 125, 279–92.Google Scholar
Power, T. D. (1965). ‘Some aspects of brain-mind relationship.British Journal of Psychiatry, 3, 1215–23.Google Scholar
Riese, W. (1958). ‘Freudian concepts of brain function and brain disease.Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 127, 287307.Google Scholar
Russell, J. R., and Nathan, P. W. (1946). ‘Traumatic amnesia.Brain, 69, 280300.Google Scholar
Selye, H. (1957). The Stress of Life. London: Longmans, Green and Co., p. 228.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1953). Social Behaviour in Animals. London: Methuen and Co., p. 115.Google Scholar
Walshe, F. M. R. (1965). Further Critical Studies in Neurology. Edinburgh: E. and S. Livingstone, p. 117.Google Scholar
Whitty, C. M. (1962). ‘The neurological basis of memory.’ in Modern Trends in Neurology. (Ed. Williams, D.), P. 323.Google Scholar
Yapp, W. B., and Bourne, G. H. (1957). The Aging of Mammalian Cells. London: Institute of Biology, p. 35.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.