Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ws8qp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T16:04:35.062Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mental and Psychomotor Speed in Depression and Mania

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 January 2018

Ivy M. Blackburn*
Affiliation:
M.R.C. Brain Metabolism Unit, Edinburgh University Department of Pharmacology, 1 George Square, Edinburgh, EH8 9JZ

Summary

Mental and psychomotor speed scores were obtained for three affectively ill groups of bipolar depression, unipolar depression and bipolar mania, and three corresponding recovered groups.

Comparisons were made among ill groups, among recovered groups, and between ill and recovered groups for each illness type. The following conclusions were drawn:

  1. 1. Though manic patients solve problems quicker than those of either depressed group, they do not differ much from normals.

  2. 2. With stress to work faster, the patients in the two depressed groups can quicken their speed of work and bring it up to the level of the manic patients, even though the latter also increase their speed.

  3. 3. On a psychomotor speed task, the bipolar depressives are significantly slower than the manics and the unipolar depressives, who do not differ. ‘Internal distraction’ causes a slight increase in speed in all three groups, with no significant difference, but ‘external distraction’ speeds up the bipolar groups and slows down the unipolar depressives, resulting in significant differences between the bipolars on the one hand and the unipolars on the other.

  4. 4. All recovered groups perform at the same level on all measures.

  5. 5. Comparison between ill and recovered groups seems to indicate that a manic illness does not affect mental or psychomotor speed significantly when tested objectively. Unipolar depression also does not seem to affect speed functions very significantly, but bipolar depression does decrease both mental and psychomotor speed significantly. Thus only bipolar depressive patients seem to show true retardation, and manic patients do not show the opposite of retardation.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1975 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Babcock, H. (1941) Time and Mind. Cambridge, Mass.: Massachussets Science and Art Publications.Google Scholar
Babcock, H. & Levy, L. (1940) The Measurement of Efficiency of Mental Functioning. Chicago: Stoelting.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Feshback, S. & Legg, D. (1962) The clinical utility of the digit-symbol test. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 26, 263—8.Google Scholar
Blackburn, I. M. (1972) A Psychometric Study of Unipolar and Bipolar Affective Disorders. Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis, Edinburgh University Library.Google Scholar
Blackburn, I. M. (1974) The pattern of hostility in affective illness. British Journal of Psychiatry, 125, 141—5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, K. A. (1949) Changes in test performance of ambulatory depressed patients undergoing electric shock therapy. Journal of General Psychiatry, 41, 195232.Google Scholar
Foulds, G. A. (1952) Temperamental differences in maze performance. Part II. The effect of distraction and electroconvulsive therapy on psychomotor retardation. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 3341.Google Scholar
Foulds, G. A. & Hope, K. (1968) Manual of the Symptom-Sign Inventory. University of London Press.Google Scholar
Furneaux, W. D. (1956) Manual of Nufferno Speed Tests. National Foundation for Educational Research.Google Scholar
Gibson, H. B. (1965) Manual of the Gibson Spiral Maze, University of London Press.Google Scholar
Jastak, J. (1949) Problems of psychometric scatter analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 46, 177—97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kear-Colwell, J. J. (1970) The B factor scale of the 16 P.F. as a measure of intelligence in psychiatric patients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 26, 477—9.3.0.CO;2-P>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, E. H. (1953) An experimental investigation of intellectual speed and power in mental disorders. Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis, University of London Library.Google Scholar
Paulson, G. W. & Gottlieb, G. (1961) A longitudinal study of the electroencephalographic arousal response in depressed patients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 133, 524—8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, R. W. & Hewlett, J. H. G. (1960) Thought disorder in psychotic patients. In Experiments in Personality, Volume II (ed. Eysenck, H. J.). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Perris, C. (1966) A study of bipolar (manic-depressive) and unipolar (recurrent depressive) psychoses. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, Supplement 194.Google Scholar
Rapaport, D. (1945) Diagnostic psychological testing: the theory, statistical evaluation and diagnostic application of a battery of tests. Chicago Yearbook, Volume I.Google Scholar
Shapiro, M. B., Campbell, D., Harris, A. & Dewsbury, J. P. (1958) Effects of ECT upon psychomotor speed and the distraction effect in depressed psychiatric patients. Journal of Mental Science, 104, 681—95.Google Scholar
Winokur, G., Clayton, P. J. & Reich, T. (1969) Manic depressive Illness. St. Louis: Mosby Co.Google Scholar
Yates, A. (1960) Abnormalities of psychomotor function. In Handbook of Abnormal Psychology (ed. Eysenck, H. J.). London: Pitman.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.