Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-r7xzm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T04:33:04.978Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Defensive Function of Persecutory Delusions

Evidence from Attribution Tasks

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Helen M. Lyon
Affiliation:
Hafed Las Community Mental Health Team, Llwar-y-Dref, Ynys Mon, Gwynedd
Sue Kaney
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology, Whelan Building, University of Liverpool, PO Box 147, Liverpool L69 3BX
Richard P. Bentall*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology, Whelan Building, University of Liverpool, PO Box 147, Liverpool L69 3BX
*
Correspondence

Abstract

Abnormalities of ‘social’ reasoning were investigated in patients suffering from persecutory delusions and in matched depressed and normal controls using transparent (obvious) and opaque (unobvious) tests of attributional style. Whereas depressed and normal subjects yielded similar causal inferences for both attributional measures, the deluded subjects showed a marked shift in internality, attributing negative outcomes to external causes on the transparent Attributional Style Questionnaire but, on the more opaque Pragmatic Inference Task, attributing negative outcomes to internal causes and thus showing a cognitive style resembling that of the depressed group. This finding, interpreted in terms of explicit versus implicit judgements, supports the hypothesis that delusions function as a defence against underlying feelings of low self-esteem.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 1994 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P. & Teasdale, J. D. (1978) Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 78, 4074.Google Scholar
Alloy, L. B. & Abramson, L. Y. (1979) Judgement of contingency in depressed and non-depressed students: sadder but wiser? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441485.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn, revised) (DSM-III-R). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T. & Beamesderfer, A. (1974) Assessment of depression: The depression inventory. In Psychological Measurements in Psychopharmacology 1: Modern Problems in Pharmacopsychiatry. (ed. Pichot, P.) Basel: Karger.Google Scholar
Bellack, A. S. & Hersen, M. (eds) (1988) Behavioural Assessment (3rd edn). Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Bentall, R. P. (1990) The syndromes and symptoms of psychosis: or why you can't play 20 questions with the concept of schizophrenia and hope to win. In Reconstructing Schizophrenia (ed. Bentall, R. P.), pp. 2360. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bentall, R. P. (1994) Cognitive biases and delusional misattributions. In The Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia (eds David, A. & Cutting, J.). Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bentall, R. P. & Kaney, S. (1989) Content specific processing and persecutory delusions: an investigation using the emotional Stroop test. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 62, 355–64.Google Scholar
Bentall, R. P., Kaney, S. & Dewey, M. E. (1991) Paranoia and social reasoning: an attribution theory analysis. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 30, 1323.Google Scholar
Berry, D. C. & Broadbent, D. E. (1988) Interactive tasks and the implicit-explicit distinction. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 251272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blashfield, K. (1984) Classification of Psychopathology: Neo-Kraepelin and Empirical Approaches. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Brennan, J. H. & Hemsley, D. R. (1984) Illusory correlations in paranoid and non-paranoid schizophrenia. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23, 225226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewin, C. (1988) Cognitive Foundations of Clinical Psychology. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Candido, C. & Romney, D. M. (1990) Attributional style in paranoid vs depressed patients. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 63, 355363.Google Scholar
Chadwick, P. & Lowe, C. F. (1990) The measurement and modification of delusional beliefs. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 225232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, L. J. & Chapman, J. P. (1988) The genesis of delusions. In Delusional Beliefs (eds Oltmanns, T. F. & Maher, B. A.), pp. 167183. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Costello, C. G. (1992) Research on symptoms versus research on syndromes: arguments in favour of allocating more research time to the study of symptoms. British Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 304308.Google Scholar
Ellis, H. D. & Young, A. W. (1990) Accounting for delusional misidentifications. British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 239248.Google Scholar
Garety, P., Hemsley, D. & Wessely, S. (1991) Reasoning in deluded schizophrenic and paranoid patients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 179, 194201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossman, L. S. & Harrow, M. (1990) Thought disorder and cognitive processes in mania. In Cognitive Bases of Mental Disorders (ed. Magaro, P. A.), pp. 134186. New York: Sage.Google Scholar
Harrow, M., Rattenbury, F. & Stoll, F. (1988) Schizophrenic delusions: an analysis of their persistence, of related premorbid ideas and three major dimensions. In Delusional Beliefs (eds Oltmanns, T. F. & Maher, B. A.), pp. 184211. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hartman, L. M. & Cashman, F. E. (1983) Cognitive-behavioural and psychopharmacological treatment of delusional symptoms: a preliminary report. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 11, 5061.Google Scholar
Hemsley, D. R. & Garety, P. A. (1986) The formation and maintenance of delusions: a Bayesian analysis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 5156.Google Scholar
Hewstone, M. (1989) Causal Attribution: From Cognitive Processes to Collective Beliefs. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Huq, S. F., Garety, P. A. & Hemsley, D. R. (1988) Probabilistic judgements in deluded and non-deluded subjects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 40A, 801812.Google Scholar
Johnson, W. G., Ross, J. M. & Mastria, M. A. (1977) Delusional behaviour: an attribution analysis of development and modification. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 421426.Google Scholar
Kaney, S. & Bentall, R. P. (1989) Persecutory delusions and attributional style. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 62, 191198.Google Scholar
Kaney, S. & Bentall, R. P. (1992) Persecutory delusions and the self-serving bias: evidence from a contingency judgement task. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 180, 773780.Google Scholar
Kaney, S., Wolfenden, M., Dewey, M. E., et al (1992) Persecutory delusions and the recall of threatening and non-threatening propositions. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31, 8587.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Glazer, W. M. & Morgenstern, H. (1983) Dimensions of delusional experience. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 466469.Google Scholar
Kinderman, P., Kaney, S. & Bentall, R. P. (1992) Paranoia and the defensive attributional style: deluded and depressed patients attributions about their own attributions. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 65, 371383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraepelin, E. (1919/73) Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia (trans. Barclay R. M.) Huntington, NY: Kriger.Google Scholar
Maher, B. A. (1974) Delusional thinking and perceptual disorder. Journal of Individual Psychology, 30, 98113.Google Scholar
Maher, B. A. & Ross, J. S. (1984) Delusions. In Comprehensive Handbook of Psychopathology (eds Adams, H. E. & Suther, P.), New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Nelson, H. E. (1982) National Adult Reading Test Manual. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar
Oltmanns, T. F. & Maher, B. A. (eds) (1988) Delusional Beliefs. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Persons, J. B. (1986) The advantages of studying psychological phenomena rather than psychiatric diagnoses. American Psychologist, 41, 12521260.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., Semmel, A., Von Baeyer, C., et al (1982) The Attributional Style Questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6, 287300.Google Scholar
Peterson, C. & Villanova, P. (1988) An expanded Attributional Style Questionnaire. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 8789.Google Scholar
Power, M. & Brewin, C. (1991) From Freud to cognitive science: a contemporary account of the unconscious. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 30, 289310.Google Scholar
Reber, A. S. (1989) Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 219235.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, M. (1965) Society and the Adolescent Self Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Sacks, O. (1987) The Man who Mistook his Wife for a Hat. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Silverstone, P. H. (1991) Low self-esteem in different psychiatric conditions. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 30, 185188.Google Scholar
Sims, A. (1988) Symptoms in the Mind. London: Bailliere Tindall.Google Scholar
Slade, P. D. & Bentall, R. P. (1988) Sensory Deception: A Scientific Analysis of Hallucination. London: Croom-Helm.Google Scholar
Strauss, J. S. (1969) Hallucinations and delusions as points on continua function. Archives of General Psychiatry, 21, 581586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tennen, H. & Herzberger, S. (1985) Attributional Style Questionnaire. In Test Critiques IV. Westport, MO: Test Corporation of America.Google Scholar
Watts, F. N., Powell, E. G. & Austin, S. V. (1973) The modification of abnormal beliefs. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 46, 356363.Google Scholar
Winters, K. C. & Neale, J. M. (1983) Delusions and delusional thinking: a review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 3, 227253.Google Scholar
Winters, K. C. & Neale, J. M. (1985) Mania and low self-esteem. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 282290.Google Scholar
Zigler, E. & Glick, M. (1988) Is paranoid schizophrenia really camouflaged depression? American Psychologist, 43, 284290.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1979) Attribution of success and failure revisited, or the motivational bias is alive and well in attribution theory. Journal of Personality, 47, 245287.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.