Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T04:21:54.190Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Does Imagined Exposure to the Consequences of Not Ritualising Enhance Live Exposure for OCD? a Controlled Study

II. Effect on Behavioural v. Subjective Concordance of Improvement

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Extract

Background

This study tested whether adding imagined exposure to live exposure would increase the concordance between behavioural and subjective improvement in obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD).

Method

46 OCD out-patients were randomly allocated to 9 weekly sessions of either combined live + imagined exposure/ritual prevention (Exi, n = 23), or only live exposure/ritual prevention (Ex, n = 23). Patients were asked to do 90 min of daily self-exposure at home (corresponding to Exi or Ex). Measures were: (A) behavioural; (B) subjective; (C) clinical global impression (CGI).

Results

After 9 weeks of treatment, improvement was greater on behavioural than subjective measures (similar for the Exi and Ex groups). At 20 weeks (3-month follow-up) each group had improved slightly more on subjective measures and slightly less on behavioural ones. Two subjective measures improved less during Exi than Ex, but this difference disappeared at follow-up. The greater difference between behavioural and subjective improvement scores in Exi than in Ex did not relate to clinical outcome at the end of treatment or follow-up.

Conclusions

Compared to live exposure alone, combined imagined plus live exposure did not enhance behavioural/subjective concordance.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 1995 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barlow, D. H., Mavissakalian, M. R. & Schofield, L. D. (1980) Patterns of desynchrony in agoraphobia: a preliminary report. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 18, 441448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boulougouris, J. C. (1977) Variables affecting outcome in obsessive-compulsive patients treated by flooding. In Treatment of Phobic and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorders (eds Boulougouris, J. C. & Rabavilas, A. D.), ch. 9, pp. 7384. Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Foa, E. B. & Goldstein, A. (1978) Continuous exposure and complete response prevention in obsessive-compulsive neurosis. Behaviour Therapy, 9, 821829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foa, E. B., Grayson, J. B., Steketee, G. S., et al (1983) Success and failure in the behavioral treatment of obsessive-compulsives. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 287297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, E. B., Blau, J. S., Prout, M., et al (1977) Is horror a necessary component of flooding (implosion)? Behavior Research and Therapy, 15, 397402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foa, E. B., Steketee, G., Turner, R. M., et al (1980) Effects of imaginai exposure to feared disasters in obsessive–compulsive checkers. Behavior Research and Therapy, 18, 449455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foa, E. B., Steketee, G., & Grayson, J. B. (1985) Imaginai and in vivo exposure: a comparison with obsessive-compulsive checkers. Behavior Therapy, 16, 292302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greist, J. H., Jefferson, J. W. & Rosenfeld, R. (1990) Clomipramine and OCD: A placebo–controlled double-blind study of 32 patients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 51, 292297.Google Scholar
Heninger, G. R. & Charney, D. S. (1989) The Yale-Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale (YBOCS). Parts I and II. Development, use, reliability and validity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46, 10061016.Google Scholar
Jenike, M. A., Hyman, S. & Baer, L. (1989) A controlled trial of fluvoxamine in OCD: Implications for a serotonergic theory. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 12091215.Google Scholar
Kasvikis, Y. & Marks, I. M. (1988) Clomipramine in obsessive-compulsive ritualisers treated with exposure therapy: Relations between dose, plasma levels, outcome and side effects. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 95, 113118.Google ScholarPubMed
Lader, M. & Marks, I. M. (1971) Clinical Anxiety. London: Heinemann.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lelliott, P. T., Noshirvani, H. F., Basoğlu, M., et al (1980) Obsessive-compulsive beliefs and treatment outcome. Psychological Medicine, 18, 696702.Google Scholar
Marks, I. M. (1965) Patterns of meaning in psychiatric patients – semantic differential responses in obsessives and psychopaths. Institute of Psychiatry: Maudsley Monographs No. 13. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Marks, I. M. (1980) Living with Fear, Ch. 12. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Marks, I. M. (1987) Fears, Phobias and Rituals. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Marks, I. M., Stern, R. S., Mawson, D., et al (1980) Clomipramine and exposure for obsessive–compulsive rituals. British Journal of Psychiatry, 136, 125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marks, I. M., Swinson, R. P., Basoğlu, M., et al (1993) Alprazolam and exposure alone and combined in panic disorder with agoraphobia: A controlled study in London and Toronto. British Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 776787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Sullivan, G. & Marks, I. M. (1990) Long-term outcome of phobic and obsessive–compulsive disorders after treatment. In Handbook of Anxiety. Vol. 4. The Treatment of Anxiety (eds Noyes, R., Roth, M. & Burrows, G. D.). Oxford: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
Piggot, T. A., Pato, M. T. & Bernstein, S. E. (1990) Controlled comparisons of clomipramine and fluoxetine in the treatment of obsessive–compulsive disorder: behavioral and biological results. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47, 926932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pocock, S. (1990) Clinical Trials. Chichester: Wiley.Google ScholarPubMed
Rabavilas, A. D., Boulougouris, J. C. & Stefanis, C. (1976) Duration of flooding sessions in the treatment of obsessive–compulsive patients. Behavior Research and Therapy, 14, 349355.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachman, S. (1976) Obsessional compulsive checking. Behavior Research and Therapy, 14, 269277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachman, S. & Hodgson, R. (1974) Synchrony and desynchrony in fear and avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 12, 311318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rachman, S., Marks, I. M. & Hodgson, R. (1973) The treatment of obsessive–compulsive neurotics by modelling and flooding in vivo. Behavior Research and Therapy, 11, 463471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
SPSS/PC + (1988) Advanced statistics V2.0 system, version 3.1. Chicago: SPSS Inc.Google Scholar
Stern, R. & Marks, I. M. (1973) Brief and prolonged flooding: A comparison in agoraphobic patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 28, 270276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, J. P. & Marks, I. M. (1971) Relevant and irrelevant fear in flooding: A cross-over study of phobic patients. Behaviour Therapy, 2, 275293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.