Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T23:28:46.945Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Establishing the limits of normal cerebral ageing and senile dementias

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 January 2018

Karen Ritchie*
Affiliation:
Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Montpellier, France
*
Karen Ritchie. INSERM CJF 97-02, Epidemiology of Neurodegenerative Diseases of the CNS. CRLC Val d'Aurelle. 34298 Montpellier Cedex 5. France. Tel: 33 046761 30 26. Fax: 33 0467 613047
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Cognitive deterioration is so commonly observed in the elderly that it is considered by many to be an inevitable feature of the ageing process. Some researchers have proposed that the senile dementias are the inevitable end-point of this process, should the person live long enough. The differentiation of normal cerebral ageing from disease process is important in the selection of control groups for research, and also for clinical decision-making. In the latter context it is important to ask at what level of dysfunction intervention should occur, and whether this should be active or palliative. Cognitive change in the elderly is here considered from biological, neuropsychological and epidemiological viewpoints. Current research suggests that senile dementia is the result of the interplay of genetically determined disease processes, ageing-related decline which may be regulated at a cellular level, and neuronal repair and compensation mechanisms. Therefore, to debate whether dementia is simply an extension of a normal ageing process or not is probably too simplistic an approach.

Type
Editorials
Copyright
Copyright © 1998 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

References

Albert, M. S. (1994) Age-related changes in cognitive function. In Clinical Neurology of Aging (eds Albert, M. L. & Knoefel, J. E.). p. 314328. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Appell, J., Kertesz, A. A Fisman, M. A. (1982) A study of functioning in Alzheimer's disease, Brain Languoge. 17, 7391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Logic, R., Breast, S., et al (1986) Dementia and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 603618.Google Scholar
Bodnar, A. G., Ouellette, M. Frolkis, M., et al (1998) Extension of life-span by introduction of telomerase into normal human cells. Science. 279, 349352.Google Scholar
Bondy, S. C. (1992) Reactive oxygen species: relation to aging and neurotoxic damage. Neurotoxicology, 13, 87100.Google Scholar
Brayne, C. & Calloway, P. (1988) Normal ageing, impaired cognitive function, and senile dementia of the Alzheimer's type: a continuum? Lancet, iv, 12651269.Google Scholar
Cotman, C. W. & Anderson, A. J. (1995) A potential role for apoptosis in neurodegeneration and Alzheimer's disease. Molecular Neurobiology. 10, 1945.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craik, H. M. (1977) Age differences in human memory. In Handbook of the Psychology of Aging (eds Birren, J. E. & Schaie, K. W.). New York: Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Crook, T., Ferris, S. H. & McCarthy, M. (1979) The misplaced-objects task: a brief test for memory dysfunction in the aged. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 27, 284287.Google Scholar
Crook, T., Bartus, R. T. & Ferris, S. H. (1986) Age-associated memory impairment: proposed diagnostic criteria and measures of clinical change. Report of a National Institute of Mental Health Workgroup. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2, 261276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Detacourte, A. & Buée, L. (1997) Normal and pathological tau proteins as factors for microtubule assembly. International Review Cytology. 171, 167224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Detacourte, A., David, J. P., Sergeant, N., et al (1998) Cerebral ageing and Alzheimer's disease: demonstration of the absence of a continuum. Alzheimer's Reports. 1, 19110.Google Scholar
Ebly, E. M., Parhad, I. M., Hogan, M. D., et al (1994) Prevalence and types of dementia in the very old: results from the Canadian study of health and aging. Neurology. 44, 15931600.Google Scholar
Esiri, M. (1997) In Psychiatry in the Elderly (eds Jacoby, R. & Oppenheimer, C.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ferris, S. H., Crook, T., Clark, E., et al (1980) Facial recognition memory deficits in normal aging and senile dementia. Journal of Gerontology. 35, 707714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fichter, M. M., Melier, I., Schröppel, H., et al (1995) Dementia and cognitive impairment in the oldest old in the community. British Journal of Psychiatry. 166, 621629.Google Scholar
Flicker, C., Ferris, S. H. & Raisberg, B. (1993) A two-year longitudinal study of cognitive function in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology. 6, 8496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomez-isla, T., Price, J., McKeel, D., et al (1996) Profound loss of layer II entorhinal cortex neurons occurs in very mild Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neuroscience. 16, 44914500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huff, F. J., Corkin, S. & Growdon, J. H. (1986) Semantic impairment and anomta in Alzheimer's disease. Brain and Language. 28, 235249.Google Scholar
Itzhaki, R. F. (1994) Possible factors in the etiology of Alzheimer's disease. Molecular Neurobiology. 19, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jagger, C., Clarke, M. & Clarke, S. J. (1991) Getting older-feeling younger: the changing health profile of the elderly International Journal of Epidemiology. 20, 234236.Google Scholar
Jellinger, K. A. (1995) Alzheimer's changes in non-demented and demented patients. Acta Neuropathologica. 89, 112113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jobst, K. A., Smith, A. D., Szatmari, M., et al (1994) Rapidly progressing atrophy of medial temporal lobe in Alzheimer's disease. Lancet, 343, 829830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johanssen, B. & Zerit, S. K. (1997) Early cognitive markers of the incidence of dementia and morality: a longitudinal population-based study of the oldest old. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 12, 5359.Google Scholar
Jonkmen, E. J. (1998) Modifications EEG au cours du vieillissement normal et dans les démences de type Alzheimer. Alzheimer Actualités. 130, 612.Google Scholar
Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E. & Henderson, A. S. (1987) The prevalence of dementia: a quantitative integration of the literature. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 76, 465479.Google Scholar
Khachaturian, Z. S. (1985) Diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. Archives of Neurology, 42, 10971105 Google Scholar
Kehn, R. R. (1982) Causes of death in very old people. Journal of the American Medical Association. 247, 27932797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leibovici, D. & Ritchie, K., (1995) Heterogeneity in senile dementia and normal cognitive ageing. Alzheimer's Research, 1, 1722.Google Scholar
Leibovici, D. & Ritchie, K., Ledésert, B., et al (1996) Does education level determine the course of cognitive decline? Age and Ageing. 25, 392397.Google Scholar
Manton, K. G., & Stallard, E. (1988) Chronic Disease Modeling, p. 101 London: Charles Griffin.Google Scholar
Martin, A. & Fedio, P. (1983) Word production and comprehension in Alzheimer's disease: the breakdown in semantic knowledge. Brain and Language. 19, 124–111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A., Brouwers, P., Lalonde, F., et al (1988) Towards a behavioural typology of Alzheimer's patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 8, 594610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayeux, R., Stern, Y. & Spanton, S. (1985) Heterogeneity in dementia of the Alzheimer type, evidence of subgroups. Neurology. 35, 453461.Google Scholar
McGee, M. & Brayne, C. (1998) The impact on prevalence of dementia in the oldest age groups of differential mortality patterns: a deterministic approach. International Journal of Epidemiology. 27, 8790.Google Scholar
Morris, R. G., (1986) Short term forgetting in senile dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 3, 7797 Google Scholar
Morrison, J. H. & Hof, P. R. (1997) Life and death of neurons in the aging brain. Science. 278, 412–119.Google Scholar
Politoff, A. L. & Monson, N. (1996) Age vs aging in the pathogenesis of senile dementia of the Alzheimer type: electrophysiological evidence. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 8, 1825 Google Scholar
Ritchie, K. (1994) International comparisons of dementia-free life expectancy: a critical review of the results obtained. In Advances in Health Expectancies (eds Mathers, C., McCallum, J. & Robine, J.-M.) Canberra: Australian Institute of Health and Welfare Google Scholar
Ritchie, K., Kildea, D. & Robine, J.-M. (1991) The relationship between age and the prevalence of senile dementia a meta-analysis of recent data. International Journal of Epidemiology. 21, 763769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ritchie, K. & Tbuchon, J. (1991) Heterogeneity in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type: individual differences, progressive deterioration of clinical sub-types? Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 45, 13911398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ritchie, K., Robine, J.-M., Letennaur, L., et al (1994) Dementia-free life expectancy in France. American Journal of Pubic Health, 84, 232236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritchie, K. & Kildea, D. (1995) Is senile dementia “age-related” or “ageing-related”? – evidence from a meta-analysis of dementia prevalence in the oldest old. Lancet. 346, 931934 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritchie, K., Leibovici, D., Ledésert, B., et al (1994) A typology of sub-clinical senescent cognitive disorder, British Journal of Psychiatry. 168, 470476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robine, J.-M., Romieu, I. & Cambois, E. (1997) Health expectancies and current research. Reviews in Clinical Gerontotogy, 7, 7381 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sahakian, B. J., Morris, R. G., Evenden, J. L., el al (1988) A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinsons disease. Brain. 111, 695718.Google Scholar
Schale, K. W. (1983) The Seattle Longitudinal Study: A twenty-one year exploration of psychometric intelligence in adulthood. In Longitudinal Studies of Adult Psychological Development (ed Scheie, K W.) New York: Guilford Press Google Scholar
Schale, K. W., & Willis, S. L. (1991) Adult personality and psychomotor performance: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Journal of Gerontology. 46, 275284.Google Scholar
Storandt, M., Morris, J. C., McKeel, D. W., et al (1994) Psychometric test distinction of normal ageing versus very mild Alzheimer's disease. In Proceedings of the international Psychogeriatric Association Workshop. pp. 8485. Amsterdam: International Psychogeriatric Association.Google Scholar
Vaidois, S., Jounette, Y., Poissant, A., et al (1990) Heterogeneity in the cognitive profile of normal elderly. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 12, 587596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wast, M. J., Coleman, P. O. Flood, D. J., et al (1994) Differences in the pattern of hippocampal neuronal loss in normal ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet. 344, 769772 Google Scholar
Wyllie, A. M. Kerr, J. F. R., Currle, A. R. (1980) Cell death the significance of apoptosis international Review of Cytology. 68, 251306 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.