Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-995ml Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T10:38:46.742Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psychiatric stigma

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Peter Byrne*
Affiliation:
Kent Institute of Medical and Health Sciences, 2–4 Radnor Park Avenue, Folkestone, Kent CT19 5HN. E-mail: byrenpr@btinternet.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © 2001 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Although there is agreement on what ‘stigma’ is (a mark of disgrace or discredit that sets a person aside from others), definitions differ in the breadth of experiences they describe. Stigmatisation is the process wherein one condition or aspect of an individual is attributionally linked to some pervasive dimension of the target person's identity (Reference Mansouri and DowellMansouri & Dowell, 1989). It is the negative effect of a label (Reference Hayward and BrightHayward & Bright, 1997), or the process of establishing deviant identities (Reference SchlosbergSchlosberg, 1993). For Corrigan & Penn (Reference Corrigan and Penn1999), stigma is another term for prejudice based on negative stereotyping. The clear inference is that the ‘negative’ aspect reflects not only unfavourable stereotypes, but also the negative attitudes and adverse behaviour of the stigmatiser. Clausen (Reference Clausen1981) saw stigma as “a buzz word, arousing more emotional reaction than words like devaluation and discrimination”. Other writers have questioned the usefulness of the word to describe a range of adverse experiences. Sayce (Reference Sayce2000) argues that the focus should move from the receiver of stigma (the psychiatric patient) to the people or agency causing the stigma. To be marked as ‘mentally ill’ carries internal (secrecy, lower self-esteem and shame) and external (social exclusion, prejudice and discrimination) consequences, all of which are written about under the ‘stigma’ heading. The stigma literature is further divided on the basis of the background or discipline of researchers and their vision of how to reduce the stigma and discrimination against people who have had, or currently endure, mental disorders. One example of this diversity, and an excellent place to begin studying this subject, is the Lancet series (1998, 352, September 26) on stigma. This series has contributions from users and professionals, with writers who are both. It coincided with the launch of the Royal College of Psychiatrists' ‘Changing Minds’ anti-stigma campaign (Reference WhiteWhite, 1998; Reference CrispCrisp, 2000; www.stigma.org).

Stories and histories

The first and last words on stigma belong to people with mental disorders. Shaw (Reference Shaw1998) quotes the knee-jerk reaction to her book about her postnatal depression: “how can we believe this person's account to be credible when she says herself she was suffering from severe depression at the time”. Thankfully, only a minority of psychiatrists ignores such narratives. The seminal work by Goffman (Reference Goffman1963) is annotated with the experiences of people who had been marked by a variety of stigmata. Barham & Hayward's project (Reference Barham and Hayward1995) to tell the stories of people with schizophrenia living in the community is reminiscent of Goffman's work with asylum inmates. Read & Reynolds (Reference Read and Reynolds1996) & Sayce (Reference Sayce2000) provide longer accounts of what it means to have a mental illness. The reality of US psychiatric careers forms the major part of Fink & Tasman (Reference Fink and Tasman1992) and Wahl (Reference Wahl1999). The common themes of all six books are social exclusion, financial hardship and discrimination. A recent report, based on responses from 556 UK users, shows that 70% have experienced discrimination in some form: 47% in the workplace, 44% from general practitioners and 32% from other health professionals (Mental Health Foundation, 2000).

For an historical perspective, the most readable book is Porter (Reference Porter1991). It is a valuable record of contemporaneous attitudes and (often indefensible) treatments. Full of highly readable personal accounts, it begins with playwright Nathaniel Lee's famous quotation: “they called me mad, and I called them mad, and damn them, they outvoted me” (Reference PorterPorter, 1991). Stigma across time and diverse cultures is also described in Section 2 of Fink & Tasman (Reference Fink and Tasman1992). The most comprehensive and scholarly work in this field is by Fabrega (Reference Fabrega1990, Reference Fabrega1991a ,Reference Fabrega b ). These articles, extensively referenced, provide valuable insights into the multiple historical, religious, ethnic and cultural influences on stigmatisation. Western societies have always linked ideas of morality and virtue with health and reason, and early Christian societies tainted madness with images of the demonic, the perverse, the promiscuous and the sinful (Reference SchlosbergSchlosberg, 1993). Knowledge of these associations is necessary to understand the origins and perpetuation of psychiatric stigma.

Historical perspectives are important, but it would be folly to regard stigma as merely an historical problem, or to assume that stigma, like the poor, will be always with us. Despite scientific advances and the rise of the medical model, stigma has not gone away (Reference Read and LawRead & Law, 1999; Reference ByrneByrne, 2000). Angermeyer et al (Reference Angermeyer, Link and Majcher-Angermeyer1987) compared perceptions of stigma of German in-patients in an isolated state hospital with those of in-patients in a large university hospital unit, where they frequently met people with general medical conditions. Against the predictions of both staff and the researchers, and controlling for diagnoses and individual stigma-reduction strategies, the university hospital group perceived higher levels of stigma: they would have preferred to be in-patients in secrecy and isolation (Reference Angermeyer, Link and Majcher-AngermeyerAngermeyer et al, 1987). Other researchers have also re-evaluated public attitudes in the light of community care (Reference Taylor and DearTaylor & Dear, 1981; Reference Brockington, Hall and LevingsBrockington et al, 1993; Wolff et al, Reference Wolff, Pathare and Craig1996a ,Reference Wolff, Pathare and Craig b ,Reference Wolff, Pathare and Craig c ). Closing the asylums — the ‘relocation of madness’ — has brought about a community backlash and the reality of former patients leading isolated community-based existences (Reference Barham and HaywardBarham & Hayward, 1995).

The psychiatric literature

Accepting that stigma plays a negative role at every stage of mental illness — presentation, diagnosis, treatment and outcome (Reference ByrneByrne, 1997) — it is surprising that psychiatric textbooks omit stigma as either subject or indexed item. This may be partly explained by the fact that psychiatric articles addressing the same range of problems do not mention stigma directly. In a review of the literature, Avison (Reference Avison1996) concluded that social networks (actual and perceived) predict the onset and recurrence of mental illnesses. Lack of social networks is, in effect, social exclusion (stigma). Manning & White (Reference Manning and White1995) reported UK employers' reluctance to hire someone with mental illness, but found more tolerant attitudes to depression than to schizophrenia or alcoholism. Using standardised measurements with matched controls, Brown et al (Reference Brown, Birtwistle and Roe1999) have reported a more unhealthy lifestyle (poor diet, smoking and lack of exercise) in a cohort of 102 people with schizophrenia. Their findings, which controlled for social class, may reflect the low priority that psychiatrists and general practitioners assign to the physical health of these patients. Levenson & Oldbrisch (Reference Levenson and Oldbrisch1993) surveyed US transplant units and reported that controlled (sic) schizophrenia was a relative contraindication to transplant among two-thirds of renal and liver teams; 63% of cardiac and liver units deemed a history of affective disorder a relative contraindication to transplantation (Reference Levenson and OldbrischLevenson & Oldbrisch, 1993). Dembling et al (Reference Dembling, Chen and Vachon1999) calculate that the median age of death is 66 years for people who had been treated by state mental health facilities, compared with 76 years for controls. These and many other similar studies are part of the stigma literature and should drive the arguments to improve current practices (Wahl (Reference Wahl1999), Sayce (Reference Sayce2000) and the Mental Health Foundation (2000) provide comprehensive listings of widespread discriminatory practices in the health services and beyond).

Ghodse et al (Reference Ghodse, Ghaffari and Bhat1986) highlight unfavourable attitudes in medical and nursing staff to patients who overdose, and these are even worse when there is perceived alcohol or drug dependence. Measuring the attitudes of health professionals, Fleming & Szmukler (Reference Fleming and Szmukler1992) found that patients with anorexia were seen as “less likeable” than other patients and as being responsible for their illness. Lefley (Reference Lefley1987) reported that 90% of mental health professionals who had a family member with mental illness, frequently heard colleagues make “negative or disparaging remarks” about patients: the majority of these professionals stayed silent and did not disclose their relative's illness (Reference LefleyLefley, 1987). Schwartz & Schwartz (Reference Schwartz and Schwartz1977) criticised psychiatry by identifying practices that contribute to stigma, principally an overinclusive definition of mental illness and overinvolvement by the profession in the justice system. Schlosberg (Reference Schlosberg1993) argued that psychiatrists, often oblivious to stigma, have a clear choice of role: to be either stigmatisers or destigmatisers of their patients. He cautioned against further stigmatisation of an “undesirable” patient group — those who have other stigmata (e.g. old age, poor educational status), are treatment resistant, or reject medical authority (Reference SchlosbergSchlosberg, 1993). The relatively new profession of psychiatry did not cause stigma, but many of its former and current practices perpetuate it (Reference ByrneByrne, 2000).

Hayward & Bright (Reference Hayward and Bright1997) reported a fall in interest in stigma publications in the 1970s, noting that this has reversed during the past decade. Previously neglected topics, for example the stigma of dementia or substance misuse, have now been addressed. Benbow & Reynolds (Reference Benbow and Reynolds2000), writing about the stigma of Alzheimer's dementia, comment about ageist attitudes and therapeutic nihilism, and the fact that once the illness was deemed psychiatric, this brought its own additional stigma. Negative social meaning placed on stigmatised behaviour (for example, addiction) can often be a more important barrier to change than the behaviour (alcohol or drug misuse) itself (Reference RitsonRitson, 1999). Ritson's article is one of a ‘stigma series’ in the International Journal of Clinical Practice. Both dementia and alcohol and drug addiction have been included in the ‘Changing Minds’ antistigma campaign (Reference CrispCrisp, 2000). Along with this resurgence in publications come broadly based articles, challenging medicine to ‘put its own house in order’ (Reference WhiteWhite, 1998). The best work in this area has been published relatively recently: Wolff et al (Reference Wolff, Pathare and Craig 1996b ,Reference Wolff, Pathare and Craig c ), in a controlled trial of community education, have shown individually tailored local initiatives as the way forward. In a related study, the same group quantified the tarnished image of community psychiatry, but also showed that there is a demand for information and education among (potentially hostile) residents' groups and argued that these concerns should be addressed (Reference Wolff, Pathare and CraigWolff et al, 1996a ).

Other perspectives

In addition to personal, historical and psychiatric perspectives, other disciplines have added to current concepts. Much of Goffman (Reference Goffman1963) is still highly relevant today, and sociologists have taken these ideas forward, especially in the literature on the nature of prejudice and stereotypes (Reference ByrneByrne, 1997). While some writers have considered attribution theory (certain negative traits can discredit the person to whom they are ascribed) to explain stigma, labelling theory (symptoms and bahaviour are seen as violations of social norms or products of situations rather than as the expressions of psychopathology) has been advocated by most (Reference SchlosbergSchlosberg, 1993). Link et al (Reference Link, Mirotznik and Cullen1991) describe individual responses to labelling (defence, attack or adaptation) as secondary deviation, believing that these efforts are bound to fail. Although not directly referenced in this review, both nursing and social work journals have consistently shown a broader interest in stigma than the psychiatric literature (Reference ByrneByrne, 2000). Anthropology and cultural studies provide a wider context within which stigma operates. Prejudice against people with mental illness has parallels with racism and racial discrimination: one particular challenge is the coincidence of mental disorder and minority ethnicity, so-called double discrimination. Ethnicity is also an important factor in anti-stigma initiatives, and Wolff et al (Reference Wolff, Pathare and Craig1996b ) reported key attitudinal differences in ethnic groups.

Social psychology has made a major contribution to stigma research, although the methodology is quite different to that of medical research. Measuring stigma remains problematic: prejudiced people tend to play down these attitudes, and behaviour is more difficult to study in real settings. Amerigo Farina has been at the forefront in fieldwork studies examining the complex precursors of adverse behaviours, and has integrated anti-stigma strategies into treatment programmes (Reference FarinaFarina, 1998). Bruce Link has also contributed to current thinking, with his modified labelling theory and work showing stigma to be at least as important as socio-economic variables in determining symptomatology and outcomes (Reference Link, Mirotznik and CullenLink et al, 1991). Angermeyer et al (Reference Angermeyer, Link and Majcher-Angermeyer1987) used Link's 29-item Perceived Stigma Questionnaire to record perceptions of and coping styles for stigma. Younger patients were more aware of the social costs of their illness, and people with higher professional status were more likely to be encouraged by mental health professionals to keep their psychiatric status secret (Reference Angermeyer, Link and Majcher-AngermeyerAngermeyer et al, 1987). Mansouri & Dowell (Reference Mansouri and Dowell1989) report that stigma is a significant source of distress in, for example, people with severe enduring mental illness in a community-support programme, where it correlates with self-esteem. Their study is unusual in that the majority of social psychology research has been conducted among volunteer students rather than users and carers. The key question is whether attitudes, and more importantly changes in attitudes and behaviour, in undergraduates can be generalised to other populations. None the less, Desforges et al (Reference Desforges, Lord and Ramsey1991) provide a useful examination of the contact hypothesis, that is, contact with someone with mental illness predicts less prejudice and more positive behaviour. Using students as subjects, they make a case that optimum contact should revolve around shared and multiple tasks (Reference Desforges, Lord and RamseyDesforges et al, 1991). Corrigan & Penn (Reference Corrigan and Penn1999) believe that contact will be productive only if there is equal status between the stigmatised and stigmatisers.

Capturing public opinion

There is no agreed instrument to measure stigma, but measuring public opinion about mental disorders is central to understanding and reducing it. Public opinion surveys identify levels of awareness, belief systems, fears and stereotypes of these disorders but they can be criticised for underestimating antipathy, as those unaffected or disinterested may refuse to participate in a survey and many give socially desirable responses. Taylor & Dear (Reference Taylor and Dear1981) have developed a valid and reliable scale, the Community Attitudes to Mental Illness (CAMI), which measures attitudes across a number of key parameters. It was developed specifically for use in the community (in the context of the emptying of the asylums) and refined to quantify attitudes across four scales: authoritarianism, benevolence, social destructiveness and community mental health ideology. Hayward & Bright (Reference Hayward and Bright1997) concluded that medical researchers tend to ask closed questions and obtain positive answers, while sociologists ask open questions, uncovering negative stigmatising answers. This is certainly untrue of recent medical research. Brockington et al (Reference Brockington, Hall and Levings1993) measured UK residents' attitudes using a modified CAMI. They related fear of those with mental illness to factors such as increased age, lower occupational class and having no acquaintance with mental illness: increased age was associated with greater benevolence, and students (under 25) had the least benevolent attitudes. Within studies of this type there are many contradictions. Benevolent attitudes (“best possible care for people who are ill”) may coexist with higher levels of fear: Wolff et al (Reference Wolff, Pathare and Craig1996b ) report mostly positive attitudes in adults with young children, but high fear and exclusion scores. Identifying specific components of public opinion is essential in directing educational initiatives. Penn et al (Reference Penn, Kommana and Mansfield1999) describe an educational programme designed specifically to address perceived and actual dangerousness, which has had encouraging results.

Another entry into understanding the nature of stigmatisation is through the media, a single but very important source of information about issues such as mental illness. Their currency is stereotype, and the common stereotypes of mental illness correspond to the four CAMI scales above (Reference ByrneByrne, 1997; Reference WahlWahl, 1999). Wilson et al (Reference Wilson, Nairn and Coverdale2000) have studied 128 children's television programmes, the majority made in the USA. Of these, 46% referred to mental illness references using derogatory terms (nuts, bananas, twisted, wacko, cuckoo, freak, etc.) and six had characters labelled consistently as ill, with no other redeeming characteristics. According to labelling theory children learn these stereotypes at an early age (Reference ByrneByrne, 1997). Read & Law (Reference Read and Law1999) comment that the mental illness stereotypes held by journalists are similar to those of mental health professionals. Clausen (Reference Clausen1981) saw media misrepresentation as perhaps the only issue in stigmatisation, but others play down its influence:

“the public shows a remarkable capacity to enjoy the cavalcade of hypocritical, frivolous or malicious entertainment served up to it… people can be intelligent and discerning, and still chose to enjoy rubbish” (Reference RadfordRadford, 1996).

McKeon (Reference McKeon1998) argues for a comprehensive stigma-prevention programme, which should challenge the negative media depictions and promote a balanced portrayal of mental illness in the media. Thus, in revealing, protesting against and reducing stigma, the media is an excellent starting point. Penn et al (Reference Penn, Kommana and Mansfield1999) devised an educational programme for young people that used written case vignettes, but these may not have the same impact as the latest tabloid headlines or ‘psychokiller’ film. Recent trends, for example, celebrities' disclosure of their depression and the twinning of mood disorder with creativity, may be counterproductive (Reference McKeonMcKeon, 1998). Equally psychiatrists ‘coming out’ may perpetuate the notion of the infectivity of mental illness. Salter & Byrne (Reference Salter and Byrne2000) argue for a greater interaction between psychiatry and television, but on the latter's terms, acting and reacting quickly by using powerful human interest stories and restoring positive images of people with mental illness. Professionals need to continue to object to media inaccuracy: one established newspaper health correspondent commented that he has had more complaints from dentists than from psychiatrists (Reference Salter and ByrneSalter & Byrne, 2000). The US-based National Alliance for the Mentally Ill website, www.nami.org includes ‘Stigma Busters’ which provides subscribers with free e-mail updates of ongoing battles for fairer media coverage.

Solution-focused work

Everyone writing about stigma, directly or indirectly, offers ready solutions, summed up by Corrigan & Penn (Reference Corrigan and Penn1999) as: protest, education and contact. We must protest at all discriminatory practices and media misrepresentations. However, in doing so we need to keep in mind the ‘rebound’ phenomenon, where suppression of prejudice, insensitive protest or foisting unrealistic stereotypes onto the public may lead to a backlash (Reference Corrigan and PennCorrigan & Penn, 1999). Educational campaigns — collaborative, culturesensitive and comprehensive — show best results when locally based and focused on the anxieties of their target groups. There is a compelling argument that psychiatrists should extend existing psychoeducational programmes into the broader context of public education (Reference ByrneByrne, 2000). But education needs to be something more than psychiatric training for the lay public. People with mental illness (who have a greater than average knowledge of mental disorder) tend to be more negative to others who are ill (Reference Hayward and BrightHayward & Bright, 1997). Read & Law (Reference Read and Law1999) believe that propagation of the medical model will perpetuate stigma: information on genes and ‘chemical imbalances’ implies that those with mental illness have no control over or responsibility for their actions. In a study of the effect of a public education campaign on mental illness, Wolff et al (Reference Wolff, Pathare and Craig1996c ) showed that even though knowledge about mental illness remained unchanged in their study group, attitudes and behavioural intention towards those with mental illness did improve. In addition to educational programmes, contact needs to be carefully planned (see above). All studies quoted in this paper strengthen the argument that the measures of success in treating psychiatric patients should extend beyond dopamine receptor occupancy and should include quality of life indices, markers of physical and psychological well-being and wider social contexts. Others take the arguments even further, and see this as a civil and human rights issue, advocating a disability inclusion model (Reference SayceSayce, 2000). Stigma may be topical and trendy, but it is more than exam fodder or a passing fashion. Ann Beall (quoted in Reference SayceSayce 2000, p. 78) wrote:

“we are truly an outcast people. Anti-stigma work is about rediscovering common ground. Otherwise we'll always be the ‘other’. We led the parade to the death camps and no-one questioned”.

References

Angermeyer, M. C., Link, B. G. & Majcher-Angermeyer, A. (1987) Stigma perceived by patients attending modern treatment settings: some unanticipated effects of community psychiatry reforms. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 411.Google Scholar
Avison, W. R. (1996) Social networks as risk and protective factors for onset and recurrence of mental disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 9, 149152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barham, P. & Hayward, R. (1995) Relocating Madness: From the Mental Patient to the Person. London: Free Association Books.Google Scholar
Benbow, S. M. & Reynolds, D. (2000) Challenging the stigma of Alzheimer's disease. Hospital Medicine, 61, 174177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brockington, I. F., Hall, P., Levings, J., et al (1993) The community's tolerance of the mentally ill. British Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 9399.Google Scholar
Brown, S., Birtwistle, J., Roe, L., et al (1999) The unhealthy lifestyle of people with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 29, 697701.Google Scholar
Byrne, P. (1997) Psychiatric stigma: past, passing and to come. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 90, 618620.Google Scholar
Byrne, P. (2000) Stigma of mental illness and ways of diminishing it. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 6, 6572.Google Scholar
Clausen, J. A. (1981) Stigma and mental disorder: phenomena and terminology. Psychiatry, 44, 287296.Google Scholar
Corrigan, P. & Penn, D. L. (1999) Lessons from social psychology on discrediting psychiatric stigma. American Psychologist, 54, 765776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crisp, A. H. (2000) Changing minds: every family in the land. An update on the College's campaign. Psychiatric Bulletin, 24, 267268.Google Scholar
Dembling, B. P., Chen, D. T. & Vachon, L. V. (1999) Life expectancy and causes of death in a population treated for serious mental illness. Psychiatric Services, 50, 10361042.Google Scholar
Desforges, D. M., Lord, C. G., Ramsey, S. L., et al (1991) Effects of structured cooperative contact on changing negative attitudes toward stigmatized social groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 531544.Google Scholar
Fabrega, H. (1990) Psychiatric stigma in the classical and medieval period: a review of the literature. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 31, 289306.Google Scholar
Fabrega, H. (1991a) The culture and history of psychiatric stigma in early modern and modern Western societies: a review of recent literature. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 32, 97119.Google Scholar
Fabrega, H. (1991b) Psychiatric stigma in non-western countries. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 32, 534551.Google Scholar
Farina, A. (1998) Stigma. In Handbook of Social Functioning in Schizophrenia (eds K. T. Mueser & N. Tarrier). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Fink, P. J. & Tasman, A. (1992) Stigma and Mental Illness. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Fleming, J. & Szmukler, G. I. (1992) Attitudes of medical professionals towards patients with eating disorders. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 26, 436443.Google Scholar
Ghodse, A. H., Ghaffari, K., Bhat, A. V., et al (1986) Attitudes of health care professionals towards patients who take overdoses. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 32, 5863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goffman, E. (1963) Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Hayward, P. & Bright, J. (1997) Stigma and mental illness: a review and critique. Journal of Mental Health, 6, 345354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lefley, H. P. (1987) Impact of mental illness in families of mental health professionals. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 613619.Google Scholar
Levenson, J. L. & Oldbrisch, M. E. (1993) Psychosocial evaluation of organ transplant candidates. Psychosomatics, 34, 314323.Google Scholar
Link, B. G., Mirotznik, J. & Cullen, F. T. (1991) The effectiveness of stigma coping orientations: can negative consequences of mental illness labelling be avoided? Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 32, 302320.Google Scholar
Manning, C. & White, P. T. D. (1995) Attitudes of employers to the mentally ill. Psychiatric Bulletin, 19, 541543.Google Scholar
Mansouri, L. & Dowell, D. A. (1989) Perceptions of stigma among the long-term mentally ill. Psychosocial Rehabilitation Journal, 13, 7991.Google Scholar
McKeon, P. (1998) Defeating or preventing the stigma of mental illness. Lancet, 352, 1942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mental Health Foundation (2000) Pull Yourself Together: A Survey of the Stigma and Discrimination Faced by People Who Experience Mental Distress. London: Mental Health Foundation.Google Scholar
Penn, D. L., Kommana, S., Mansfield, M. et al (1999) Dispelling the stigma of schizophrenia. II. The impact of information on dangerousness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25, 437446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Porter, R. (1991) Faber Book of Madness. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Radford, T. (1996) Influence and power of the media. Lancet, 347, 15331535.Google Scholar
Read, J. & Law, A. (1999) The relationship between causal beliefs and contact with users of mental health services to attitudes to the ‘mentally ill’. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 45, 216229.Google Scholar
Read, J. & Reynolds, J. (1996) Speaking Our Minds – An Anthology. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Ritson, E. B. (1999) Alcohol, drugs and stigma. International Journal of Clinical Practice, 53, 549551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salter, M. & Byrne, P. (2000) The stigma of mental illness: how you can use the media to reduce it. Psychiatric Bulletin, 24, 281283.Google Scholar
Sayce, L. (2000) From Psychiatric Patient to Citizen. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Schlosberg, A. (1993) Psychiatric stigma and mental health professionals (stigmatizers and destigmatizers). Medicine and Law, 12, 409416.Google Scholar
Schwartz, R. A. & Schwartz, I. K. (1977) Reducing the stigma of mental illness. Diseases of the Nervous System, 38, 101103.Google Scholar
Shaw, F. (1998) Mistaken identity. Lancet, 352, 10501051.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. M. & Dear, M. J. (1981) Scaling community attitudes toward the mentally ill. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 7, 225240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wahl, O. F. (1999) Telling is Risky Business – Mental Health Consumers Confront Stigma. New Brunswick: Rutger's University Press.Google Scholar
White, P. (1998) Changing Minds: banishing the stigma of mental illness. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 91, 509510.Google Scholar
Wilson, C., Nairn, R., Coverdale, J. et al (2000) How mental illness is portrayed in children's television. A prospective study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 440443.Google Scholar
Wolff, G., Pathare, S., Craig, T., et al (1996a) Who's in the lion's den? The community's perception of community care for the mentally ill. Psychiatric Bulletin, 20, 6871.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolff, G., Pathare, S., Craig, T., et al (1996b) Community attitudes to mental illness. British Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 183190.Google Scholar
Wolff, G., Pathare, S., Craig, T., et al (1996c) Public education for community care: A new approach. British Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 441447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.