Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T07:18:03.132Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive impairment in bipolar disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

J. S. Rubinsztein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry University of Cambridge, Box 189, Addenbrooke's Hospital, Hills Road, Cambridge CB2 2QQ, UK
B. J. Sahakian
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry University of Cambridge, Box 189, Addenbrooke's Hospital, Hills Road, Cambridge CB2 2QQ, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2002 

We write to correct some misconceptions evident in the recent editorial by Ferrier & Thompson (Reference Ferrier and Thompson2002). Previously, we reported impairment in accuracy measures on recognition memory tasks and increased latencies on executive tasks in patients with bipolar disorder in remission (Reference Rubinsztein, Michael and PaykelRubinsztein et al, 2000). Ferrier & Thompson (Reference Ferrier and Thompson2002) argue that the cognitive impairment observed in our study may have been confounded by the effects of ‘residual’ symptoms. As yet there is no generally accepted ‘cut-off’ for what constitutes remission. We devised rigorous criteria to define remission based on a patient's own view of his or her illness, that of their psychiatrist and a structured interview. We excluded patients with scores of ≥8 on both the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD) and Young Mania Scale (YMS). These rating scales were devised to rate symptom severity in patients with an affective disorder and not for use in normal control subjects. Our average reported score on the HRSD was 2.1 (s.e.m.=0.5) and on the YMS it was 0.8 (s.e.m.=0.4). Thus, very few residual symptoms were evident and these scores certainly do not support any concern that patients had residual depression or mania.

Although the rationale for using such scales in controls is dubious, for the sake of argument we have reanalysed our data reported in Rubinsztein et al (Reference Rubinsztein, Michael and Paykel2000) using a partial correlation analysis, as in Clark et al (2002), to control for differences observed on the HRSD (we did not rate control subjects using the mania scale) on the tests that showed significant impairment by analysis of variance (ANOVA). We still find significant impairment on both the visual recognition memory tasks and on latency measures from the one-touch Tower of London planning task (see Table 1).

Table 1 Results of partial correlation analysis on tests in which ANOVAs were significant

Dependent variable Partial correlation coefficients P
Pattern recognition memory Proportion correct 0.41 0.02
Spatial recognition memory Proportion correct 0.31 0.07
Delayed matching to sample Proportion correct 0.35 0.04
One-touch Tower of London Response time -0.42 0.02
Affective shifting task Response time 0.04 0.81

These findings suggest that there are trait impairments in accuracy of visual recognition memory and slower responses on a planning task in bipolar remission. Importantly, impairments of memory and learning have been consistently observed in a number of other recent studies where rigorous diagnostic criteria for remission were applied (e.g. Reference Van Gorp, Altshuler and ThebergeVan Gorp et al, 1998; Reference Krabbendam, Honig and WiersmaKrabbendam et al, 2000; Reference Cavanagh, van Beck and MuirCavanagh et al, 2002) as well as in a recent unpublished study (L. Clark, personal communication, 2002) that showed that verbal recall was still impaired following partial correlation for residual symptoms. The presence of significant impairments on executive tasks in bipolar remission has been more variable and may depend on clinical factors or the specific neuropsychological test paradigm employed. The precise functional significance of the cognitive impairment in bipolar remission needs to be examined further but may well impact on response to psychological and drug treatments. Cognitive symptoms could in fact be among the most sensitive indicators of incomplete remission.

References

Cavanagh, J. T. O., van Beck, M., Muir, W., et al (2002) Case – control study of neurocognitive function in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder: an association with mania. British Journal of Psychiatry 180, 320326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, L., Iversen, S. D. & Goodwin, G. M. (2000) Sustained attention deficit in bipolar disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 313319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrier, I. N. & Thompson, J. M. (2002) Cognitive impairment in bipolar affective disorder: implications for the bipolar diathesis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 293295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krabbendam, L., Honig, A., Wiersma, I., et al (2000) Cognitive dysfunction and white matter lesions in patients with bipolar disorder in remission. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 101, 274280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubinsztein, J. S., Michael, A., Paykel, E. S., et al (2000) Cognitive impairment in remission in bipolar affective disorder. Psychological Medicine, 30, 10251036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Gorp, W. G., Altshuler, L., Theberge, D. C., et al (1998) Cognitive impairments in euthymic bipolar patients with and without prior alcohol dependence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 4145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Results of partial correlation analysis on tests in which ANOVAs were significant

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.