Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-hgkh8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T05:04:39.232Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for the treatment of depression

Systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

José Luis R. Martin*
Affiliation:
Iberoamerican Cochrane Centre, Hospital de la Santa Crue i Sant Pau, Barcelona, Spain
Manuel J. Barbanoj
Affiliation:
Department of Pharmacology, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Barcelona, Spain
Thomas E. Schlaepfer
Affiliation:
University Hospital Bern, Switzerland and The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland, USA
Elinor Thompson
Affiliation:
Iberoamerican Cochrane Centre, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Barcelona, Spain
Víctor Pérez
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Barcelona, Spain
Jaime Kulisevsky
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Barcelona, Spain
*
José Luis Rodríguez Martin, Centro Cochrane Iberoamericano, Servei d'Epidemiologia Clínica i Salut Pública, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Universidad Autónoma de Barcelona UAB, Casa de Convalesccència, C/171 Sant Antoni Ma Claret, Barcelona 08041, Spain. E-mail: jrodriguezma@hsp.santpau.es
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) may be useful in the treatment of depression but results from trials have been inconclusive to date.

Aims

To assess the efficacy of rTMS in treating depression.

Method

We conducted a systematic review of randomised controlled trials that compared rTMS with sham in patients with depression. We assessed the quality of design of all studies and conducted a metaanalysis of data from trials with similar rTMS delivery.

Results

We included a total of 14 trials. The quality of the included studies was low. Pooled analysis using the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression showed an effect in favour of rTMS compared with sham after 2 weeks of treatment (standardised mean difference=–0.35; 95% CI –0.66 to –0.04), but this was not significant at the 2-week follow-up (standardised mean difference= — 0.33; 95% CI —0.84 to 0.17).

Conclusions

Current trials are of low quality and provide insufficient evidence to support the use of rTMS in the treatment of depression.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © 2003 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) is a non-invasive technique used to stimulate the human brain in vivo using very strong, pulsed magnetic fields. The technique involves the delivery of a magnetic pulse to the cortex of a subject through a hand-held stimulating coil applied directly to the head. The magnetic pulses pass unimpeded through the skull and induce an electrical current in the underlying tissue, which in turn is able to depolarise neurons (Hallet, 2000). Given its relative non-invasiveness, its potential to stimulate very focused areas of the brain and indications that it may have therapeutic effects in neuropsychiatric disorders, especially affective disorders (Reference George, Lisonby and SackeimGeorge et al, 1999), rTMS has been the focus of considerable research and clinical interest in recent years. Clinical interest originates mainly from about 15 placebo-controlled clinical studies involving around 200 subjects with depressive disorder or bipolar disorder in the depressed phase. It has been shown that rTMS has effects on the brain (Reference Ji, Schlaepfer and AizenmanJi et al, 1998; Reference Keck, Sillaber and EbnerKeck et al, 2000), but whether its properties are clinically useful and constitute meaningful alternatives to currently available treatments remains to be determined. Today rTMS presents an interesting and potentially promising technique but to our knowledge there has been no systematic evaluation of its efficacy using meta-analysis techniques. We undertook a systematic review of all available randomised trials and conducted a meta-analysis of relevant data to assess the efficacy of rTMS in treating depression.

METHOD

Identification of studies

We searched Medline (1966–March 2002), Embase (1974–March 2002). PsycLit (1980–2001), the Register of Clinical Trials of the Cochrane Collaboration Depression, Neurosis and Anxiety Review Group (January 2002) and the Cochrane Controlled Trials Register (January 2002) using the search terms MAGNETIC-STIMULATION, TMS, rTMS, DEPRESSION, DEPRESSIVE DISORDER and DYSTHMIC DISORDER and included papers published in all languages. Where possible, we contacted authors of identified randomised controlled trials (RCTs) for additional information or other relevant studies.

Inclusion criteria

Studies included were randomised trials that compared rTMS given at any frequency and at any localisation with a sham intervention in patients of any age and gender with a diagnosis of depression (depressive disorders or bipolar disorders in depressed phase), with or without psychotic symptoms according to either DSM–IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) or ICD–10 (World Health Organization, 1993).

Selection procedure, data extraction and quality assessment

Potentially relevant studies were obtained, examined independently and quantitative and qualitative data were extracted independently using a standard form.

Our quality assessment of the studies addressed three main criteria: adequate concealment of randomisation; intention-to-treat analysis; and blinding. To assess the adequacy of randomisation concealment, we looked for evidence from the study report of robust concealment of group allocation, such as a centralised system or a process in which allocations were pre-numbered, coded and kept in locked files or in sequentially numbered, sealed, opaque envelopes (Reference Clarke and OxmanClarke & Oxman, 2001). For intention-to-treat analysis we looked for evidence that all patients initially randomised had been included in the analysis, regardless of whether they had completed the study or not. We also looked for post-treatment follow-up. With respect to blinding, for practical reasons the professional giving the rTMS intervention itself (whether active rTMS or sham), cannot be blinded. If the patients had been blinded to the treatment allocation, and the outcomes had been assessed either by an assessor who was also blinded to the allocation or by the patient themselves, we classified the trial as being single blind with evaluation by external assessors.

We also looked at details of each trial design and noted whether there had been factors such as concurrent medication or therapeutic setting that may have influenced health outcomes and consequently the apparent performance of the interventions.

Outcome measures

The main outcome measure was remission of symptoms, determined by any of the following measures: time to adjunctive treatment; readmission to hospital or hospital discharge; time off work; or appropriate psychometric scales. Acceptability of treatment (as measured by withdrawals from trial) was considered as a secondary outcome.

Data synthesis

We undertook a methodological quality assessment of all the included studies. We conducted a pooled analysis of data from those trials in which the intervention given was homogeneous (same localisation, frequency and duration of treatment), using scores from the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD; Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1960, Reference Hamilton1967), because this psychometric scale was the only outcome measure that was reported by all the studies. In addition we conducted a second pooled analysis of data from those studies with homogeneous interventions that had used the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Reference Beck, Ward and MendelsonBeck et al, 1961) as a secondary outcome measure. A third pooled analysis was conducted for treatment and acceptability (measured by withdrawals).

In the cross-over studies we excluded a possible carry-over effect between the different phases of the trials by using information only from the first phase (Reference JadadJadad, 1998). For continuous data the studies included in the pooled analysis were tested for statistical homogeneity using a χ2 test and, because homogeneity was found, the pooled standardised mean difference was calculated under a fixed-effect model weighted by the inverse variance method (Cochrane Collaboration, 2000; Reference Sutton, Abrams and JonesSutton et al, 2000). For binary outcomes the relative risks were calculated using a Mantel–Haenszel fixed-effect model (Cochrane Collaboration, 2000; Reference Sutton, Abrams and JonesSutton et al, 2000) and 95% confidence intervals were calculated. Standardised mean difference (Reference Geddes, Freemantle and MasonGeddes et al, 2002) rather than weighted mean difference was used in the pooled analysis to take account of the different versions of HRSD and BDI used in the different studies.

RESULTS

We identified 85 references, from which we excluded 48 (see Appendix 1 and Fig. 1): seventeen trials with no control group (Höflich et al, 1993; George et al, 1995, 1998; Geller et al, 1997; Epstein et al, 1998; Feinsod et al, 1998; Figiel et al, 1998; García et al, 1998; Menkes et al, 1999; Nahas et al, 1999; Pridmore, 1999; Pridmore et al, 1999; Reid & Pridmore, 1999; Schouten et al, 1999; Triggs et al, 1999; Conca et al, 2000; Zheng, 2000); nine review articles (Markwort et al, 1997; George et al, 1999a, b; Pridmore & Belmaker, 1999; Tormos et al, 1999; Hansen, 2000; Krystal et al, 2000; Szuba et al, 2000; Walter et al, 2001); seven trial reports whose efficacy outcomes had been published elsewhere (Koppi et al, 1997; Teneback et al, 1999; Kozel et al, 2000; Little et al, 2000; Speer et al, 2000, Reference Speer, Kimbrell and WassermannSpeer et al, 2000; Loo et al, 2001; Moser et al, 2002); nine studies on healthy volunteers (George et al, 1996; Pascual-Leone et al, 1996a ; Bohning et al, 1999; Clark et al, 2000; D'Alfonso et al, 2000; Mosimann, 2000; Loo et al, 2000; Habel et al, 2001); one with no report of any randomisation process (Stikhina et al, 1999); one descriptive study (Turnier-Shea et al, 1999); two with outcomes other than depression (Grisaru et al, 1998; Nahas et al, 2000); and two in which rTMS was given after a previous intervention of sleep deprivation (Eichhammer et al, 2002; Padberg et al, 2002). We also excluded a further sixteen studies that were either still in progress without completed quantitative data available or for which we are awaiting data (Shajahan, 2000; Woodruff, 2000; the Avery–George–Hotzheimer database of rTMS Depression Studies, at http://www.ists.unibe.ch/ists/TMSAvery.htm).

Fig. 1 Process of inclusion of studies for review and analysis: rTMS, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation; HRSD, Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; ECT, electroconvulsive therapy; mt, motor threshold; *some studies of 2 weeks' duration measured outcomes at both 1 and 2 weeks.

We excluded four RCTs and one control clinical trial (CCT) from the identified trials because there was either no sham comparison group (Conca et al, 1996; Grunhaus et al, 2000; Pridmore, 2000; Pridmore et al, 2000) or because, although a sham group was included alongside two randomly allocated active treatment groups the sham group itself had not been generated by a randomisation process (Kolbinger et al, 1995). A detailed analysis of these five studies was included as part of a wider systematic review (Reference Martin, Barbanoj and SchlaepferMartin et al, 2002).

Among the remaining sixteen randomised controlled studies there was clinical heterogeneity with respect to four variables: localisation of rTMS application (left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, right prefrontal cortex, vertex or multiple sites; frequency of rTMS (high or low); duration of treatment–10 consecutive working days (2 weeks) or 5 consecutive working days (1 week); and number of interventions a day (one or more) (see Fig. 1). Two studies (Pascual-Leone et al, 1996b ; Speer et al, 2001) are awaiting evaluation of design data and methodological quality to be included, and additional quantitative information is needed for these studies to be analysed.

A total of thirteen published (George et al, 1997, 2000; Avery et al, 1999; Kimbrell et al, 1999; Klein et al, 1999; Loo et al, 1999; Padberg et al, 1999; Berman et al, 2000; Eschweiler et al, 2000; García-Toro et al, 2001a , b ; Manes et al, 2001; Szuba et al, 2001) and one study in preparation (further details available from the authors upon request) met the inclusion criteria for assessing the effectiveness of rTMS v. a sham intervention (see Appendix 2). The majority of these studies (13/14) compared left-sided, high-frequency rTMS (left–high) with a group receiving sham, whereas one study (Klein et al, 1999) compared right-sided, low-frequency rTMS for 2 weeks (right–low–2). Treatment duration was for 2 weeks in nine of the left–high studies (left–high–2) and for 1 week (left–high–1) in the remaining three studies. Among the 12 left–high studies, all used the HRSD as a primary indicator of efficacy, whereas nine (seven with available data) also used the BDI as a secondary outcome. A quantitative analysis of pooled data from the left–high–1 and left–high–2 studies on each of these outcome scales was possible. Two studies (Kimbrell et al, 1999; Padberg et al, 2002) included a third left-sided, low-frequency (left–low) comparison arm. Because of differences in the nature of the intervention applied with respect to localisation and frequency, these two comparisons (left–low–1 and left–low–2) were not included in the quantitative analysis, but were included in the qualitative review along with the one right–low–2 study and with one study that compared different doses of rTMS per day (Szuba et al, 2000).

Study populations

The mean age of study participants ranged from 41.8 to 60.87 years and the ratio of males to females ranged from 0.09 to 2.33 (Table 1). Thirteen of fourteen studies included in the qualitative review recruited patients who only fulfilled the criteria for major depression or major depressive illness as classified by DSM–IV criteria. Only one study recruited patients with criteria that included minor depression (Manes et al, 2001). Some studies recruited only patients with unipolar depression whereas others also recruited patients with bipolar depression in the depressed phase. Almost all studies specified that patients who were at a high risk of suicide and/or possible risk of convulsions were excluded.

Table 1 Summary of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) placebo-controlled trials

Study Participants Intervention
Author Design n (withdrawals) Pathology Mean age (s.d.) Male/female Type1 Duration of treatment period (weeks)2
Eschweiler et al, 2000 Cross-over 12 (3) Major depression (DSM—IV) 57 (8) 4/8 Left-side 10 Hz, 90% motor threshold, 20 tps of 10 s 1 (first phase)
Padberg et al, 1999 Parallel 18 (0) Major depression (DSM—IV) 51.2 (16.1) 7/11 Left-side 10 or 0.3 Hz, 90% motor threshold, 5 or 10 tps of 5 s 1
George et al, 1997 Cross-over 12 (0) Major depression (DSM—IV) 41.8 (12.4) 1/11 Left-side 20 Hz, 80% motor threshold, 20 tps of 2 s 2 (first phase)
Avery et al, 1999 Parallel 6 (0) Major depression or bipolar disorder (depressed phase) (DSM—IV) 44.5 (8.48) 1/5 Left-side 10 Hz, 80% motor threshold, 20 tps of 5 s 2 (+2 follow-up)
Berman et al, 2000 Parallel 20 (3) Major depressive episode (DSM—IV) 42.3 (10.1) 14/6 Left-side, 20 Hz, 80% motor threshold, 20 tps of 2 s 2
García-Toro et al, 2001b Parallel 28 (3 TMS, 3 sham) Major depression (DSM—IV) TMS 43.2 (13.1). Sham 45.0 (18.3) 10/12 Left-side, 20 Hz, 90% motor threshold, 30 tps of 2 s 2 (+2 follow-up)
García-Toro et al, 2001a Parallel 40 (3 TMS, 2 sham) Major depression (DSM—IV) TMS 51.5 (15.9). Sham 50.0 (11.0) 20/15 Left-side, 20 Hz, 90% motor threshold, 30 tps of 2 s 2 (+2 follow-up)
George et al, 2000 Parallel 32 (2) Major depression or bipolar disorder (depressed phase) (DSM—IV) 44.5 (8.4) 11/19 Left-side, 5 or 20 Hz, 100% motor threshold, 40 tps of 8 or 2 s 2
Kimbrell et al, 1999 Cross-over 8 (0) Major depression (DSM—IV) 42.46 (15) 6/7 Left-side, 20 Hz, 80% motor threshold, 20 tps of 2 s 2 (first phase)
Loo et al, 1999 Parallel 18 (0) Major depressive episode (DSM—IV) TMS 45.7 (14.7). Sham 50.9 (14.7) 9/9 Left-side, 10 Hz, 110% motor threshold, 30 tps of 5 s 2
Mosimann et al, in preparation3 Parallel 24 (0) Major depression (DSM—IV) 60.87 (13.25) 8/16 Left-side, 20 Hz, 100% motor threshold, 40 tps of 2 s 2
Klein et al, 1999 Parallel 70 (3) Major depression (DSM—IV) 58.2 (17.2) 17/53 Right-side, 1 Hz, 110% motor threshold, 2 tps of 60 s 2
Manes et al, 2001 Parallel 20 (0) Major or minor depression (DSM—IV) 60.7 (9.8) 10/10 Left-side, 20 Hz, 80% motor threshold, 20 tps of 2 s 1 (+1 follow-up)
Szuba et al, 2001 Parallel 16 (2) Major depression (DSM—IV) TMS 39.7 (12.1). Sham 33.4 (9.3) 6/8 Left-side, 10 Hz, 100% motor threshold, 20 tps of 5 s 2

Quality of included studies

Most of the studies were of low methodological quality. Apart from one study with 70 patients (Klein et al, 1999), the rest used sample sizes of 6–40 patients (median=19).

Randomisation

Most studies gave only general descriptions of the randomisation process and none described the methods of concealing allocation. One study (Klein et al, 1999) described only the generation of the allocation sequence through randomised lists of numbers generated by a computer program and another (further details available from the authors upon request) reported a ‘generation of randomised numbers’ without giving details of the allocation process involved.

Intention-to-treat

Although there were withdrawals from six of the included thirteen studies, only two studies (Berman et al, 2000; Eschweiler et al, 2000) undertook an intention-to-treat analysis by including the last observation carried forward in the analysis. Three studies (Avery et al, 1999; García-Toro et al, 2001a , b ) included a period of post-treatment follow-up of 2 weeks, one study included a period of post-treatment follow-up of 1 week (Manes et al, 2001) and another study (Eschweiler et al, 2000) included a period of post-treatment follow-up of 1 week between the first and second phase of the cross-over design. One study used the post-treatment follow-up for only one patient who responded totally and for three who responded partially, but it did not report on the rest of the patients treated in the study (Berman et al, 2000).

Blinding

Although most of the studies stated that they were double blind or double masked, they were, more accurately, single blind with evaluation by external assessors. Nine (seven with available data) studies also used the BDI, in which the patients themselves evaluated their response to treatment.

Confounding factors

Only three (Berman et al, 2000; Manes et al, 2001; Szuba et al, 2001) of the further studies stated that the patients were all free of psychotic medication for 1 week before the study and during the study period itself. In seven of fourteen studies the patients were described as medication resistant (failed at least one trial of pharmacological therapy during the current depressive episode) but in some cases pharmacological treatments were continued and in some cases not (Table 2).

Table 2 Patients' medication regimens during the studies

Eschweiler et al, 2000 1 All patients received constant doses of at least one antidepressant, five patients also received neuroleptics, four received benzodiazepines and two received lithium
Padberg et al, 1999 Three patients remained unmedicated. Last antidepressant was kept at a stable dose for 15 patients
George et al, 1997 1 Antidepressant medication tapered for 9 patients. Three patients had experienced a partial response to a 10-week, stable-dose antidepressant trial; these regimens were continued
Avery et al, 1999 Patients taking stable, ineffective doses of medication for at least 6 weeks continued on the same dose during the study. Patients on no medication for at least 6 weeks continued on no medication during the study
Berman et al, 2000 All patients free of antidepressants, neuroleptics and benzodiazepines for the week prior to initialising rTMS treatment, with tapers beginning earlier as necessary. Chloral hydrate given when required for insomnia to in-patients
García-Toro et al, 2001b 1 All underwent I week wash-out, off all medications. All patients started on sertraline (50 mg for 2 weeks, later increased, if necessary, depending on clinical response) at same time as rTMS treatment. All except two taking benzodiazepines at study entry
García-Toro et al, 2001a Patients had taken the same antidepressant medication for 6 weeks before study and continued this throughout the study
George et al, 2000 1 Patients free of antidepressant medications for at least 2 weeks before study entry, three patients with bipolar disorder required ongoing mood stabilisers or benzodiazepines for anxiety
Kimbrell et al, 1999 1 Nine medication-free patients with unipolar depression; one patient with bipolar II disorder, on lithium; three with bipolar I disorder, one on lithium and carbamazepine, one on lithium and lamotrigine and one medication-free. Patients with bipolar disorder who had had previous relapse during a major depressive episode when on mood stabilisers remained on these medications
Loo et al, 1999 Five patients had antidepressants withdrawn 5 days before rTMS. Patients on steady doses of antidepressants failing to show an effect were maintained for 2 weeks before and throughout the study; nine patients received venlafaxine; four received nefazadone
Mosimann et al, in preparation2 Antidepressant medication not an exclusion criterion. Dose stable for at least 2 weeks and no new psychoactive drug started for at least 6 weeks before the first rTMS
Klein et al, 1999 1 Patients: none were treatment-resistant. Patients were maintained with their previous medication regimen throughout the course of the study
Manes et al, 2001 All patients were withdrawn over 5 days from all antidepressant medications. They were drug-free 4 days before treatment
Szuba et al, 2001 1 Patients were free of any psychotropic medication for 7 days before study treatment (30 days for fluoxetine, monoamine oxidase inhibitor antidepressants or neuroleptics) or electroconvulsive treatment in the previous 30 days

Although most studies stated that they excluded patients at a high risk of suicide, some studies recruited only out-patients (George et al, 1997, 2000; Avery et al, 1999; Manes et al, 2001; further details available from the authors upon request), others recruited only in-patients (Klein et al, 1999; Loo et al, 1999), some both in-patients and out-patients (Berman et al, 2000; García-Toro et al, 2001b ) and others did not specify (Kimbrell et al, 1999; Padberg et al, 1999; Eschweiler et al, 2000; García-Toro et al, 2001a ; Szuba et al, 2001).

Quantitative analyses

Repetitive TMS (left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and high frequency) v. sham TMS

Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression. Twelve studies contributed to this analysis, giving an overall sample of 217 patients (119 in the treatment group and 98 in the placebo group). A subgroup analysis was conducted by duration of treatment (1 or 2 weeks) and for those studies that included follow-up data (at 1 or 2 weeks). After 2 weeks of treatment the standardised mean difference (SMD) for rTMS (left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, high frequency) v. sham TMS was –0.35 (95% CI –0.66 to –0.04; P=0.03; n=9), showing a difference in favour of rTMS. For those studies that reported data after 1 week of treatment or only gave treatment for 1 week, the SMD for rTMS (left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, high frequency) v. sham rTMS was not significant, at –0.18 (95% CI –0.64 to 0.27; P=0.4; n=5). After 1 week of post-treatment follow-up, the SMD was 0.08 (95% CI –0.64 to –0.81; P=0.8; n=2). After 2 weeks of post-treatment follow-up, the SMD was not statistically significant: –0.33 (95% CI –0.84 to 0.17; P=0.2; n=3) (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2 Size of effect (remission of symptoms), in the fixed-effect model, of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS, left localisation and high frequency), compared with sham rTMS for depression on the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; subgroup analyses by period of time. SMD, standardised mean difference. 1. Further details available from the authors upon request.

Beck Depression Inventory. Seven studies contributed to this analysis, giving an overall sample size of 145 patients (81 in the treatment group and 64 in the placebo group). No difference between rTMS and sham TMS was shown for any of the time periods. After 1 week of treatment, the SMD for the rTMS over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and high-frequency v. sham TMS was 0.18 (95% CI –0.47 to 0.82; P=0.6; n=3). The SMD after 2 weeks of treatment was –0.24 (95% CI –0.58 to 0.11; P=0.18; n=6). The SMD after 2 weeks of post-treatment follow-up was –0.06 (95% CI –0.56 to 0.43; P=0.8; n=3) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3 Size of effect (remission of symptoms), in the fixed-effect model, of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS, left localisation and high frequency) compared with sham rTMS for depression on the Beck Depression Inventory; subgroup analyses by period of time. SMD, standardised mean difference. 1. Further details available from the authors upon request.

The analyses were repeated using a random-effects model, but this did not alter the results.

Acceptability of treatment. Four studies (left–high–2) reported withdrawals of patients during the intervention period, with a total sample size of 114 patients (63 in the treatment group and 51 in the placebo group). The relative risk, using a fixed-effect model for rTMS v. sham rTMS for all patients was 0.88 (95% CI 0.37 to 2.13; P=0.8), which is a statistically non-significant difference.

DISCUSSION

Efficacy

We found that there is currently insufficient evidence to suggest that rTMS is effective in the treatment of depression and that the trials conducted to date have been of relatively low quality. These results do not, however, exclude the possibility that the intervention may be of benefit.

Although the SMD between active treatment and sham groups was significant in favour of the active group when measured by the HRSD immediately after 2 weeks of treatment with left-sided, high-frequency rTMS, this difference was not corroborated by a significant difference in the BDI. Furthermore, analysis of the results of those studies that tested patients at 2 weeks after the intervention period showed that any differences between the two groups had disappeared. Equally, analysis of data from trials that provided results after 1 week of treatment showed no significant effect.

Methodological considerations

The included studies all had serious methodological weaknesses. Of particular note was the small sample size (median=19), a factor that is known to introduce bias because uncontrolled variables that may influence outcomes may not be sufficiently evenly distributed between treatment and control groups (Reference ColtonColton, 1974). In all except three of the studies, all or a proportion of the patients (in both treatment and control groups) enrolled in the trials were on some form of psychotropic medication. Although, in some cases, the authors stated that patients were ‘medication resistant’ the definition of resistant was unclear and the potential for concurrent medication to interfere with the possible performance of the rTMS intervention cannot be discounted.

None of the included studies provided information in the published report on the method of allocation concealment used. One of the major sources of selection bias in randomised trials is failure to conceal adequately the group to which a particular patient has been assigned until after that patient's eligibility for the trial has been assessed (Reference Clarke and OxmanClarke & Oxman, 2001). Indeed, lack of allocation concealment has been reported to cause more bias than other components of the allocation process (Reference Schulz, Chalmers and HayesSchulz et al, 1995). For example, in certain RCTs the patients who are most likely to respond are included only in the active treatment arm (Reference Berger and ExnerBerger & Exner, 1999).

The person who gives an intervention such as rTMS obviously cannot be blinded as to whether they are actually administering the active treatment or a sham intervention. It is therefore more accurate to consider the trials as having been single blind with an evaluation by external blinded assessors (Reference Martin and Casado ColladoMartin & Casado Collado, 2002). Although, as Day (Reference Day2000) has observed, blinding of outcome assessment may be more important than blinding administration, there is nevertheless potential for patients to guess their group allocation through non-verbal (albeit unintentional) communication with the administrator of the intervention. A further threat to the efficacy of the blind arises from the nature of the sham intervention. As recent authors have commented (Reference Wassermann and LisanbyWassermann & Lisanby, 2001), depending on the way in which the sham is delivered, the physical sensation experienced can differ when receiving sham and active treatment, effectively unblinding the patient.

Measurement of treatment outcomes in depression is difficult and most clinical studies have made use of scales or inventories, of which the most common is the HRSD, based on a semi-structured interview. Some authors (Reference Hotopf, Churchill and LewisHotopf et al, 1999) have reported that rating scales based on semi-structured interviews are more susceptible to observation bias than are self-applied questionnaires such as the BDI. The lack of consistency in effect as determined by the two scales–a positive result after 2 weeks of treatment as measured by the HRSD and a negative result for the BDI–makes definitive conclusions about the nature of the change in mood of the patients impossible. Because of difficulties with interpreting results from psychometric scales (Reference RosenbergRosenberg, 2000) and the subjective or unstable character of this psychopathology, the use of other more objective outcome measures such as readmissions to hospital, time to hospital discharge, time to adjunctive treatment and time off work should be taken into account in the assessment of rTMS in the treatment of depression.

The complexity of the possible combinations for administering rTMS makes the comparison of like with like particularly difficult. For our meta-analysis we categorised the three main variations in administration method: localisation of the intervention on the skull; frequency given; and the duration of the treatment period. In the majority of included studies, rTMS was applied to the left dorsolateral frontal cortex, but it has been pointed out recently that the method for precisely targeting the stimulation in this area is inherently unreliable (Reference Wassermann and LisanbyWassermann & Lisanby, 2001). Evidence that this is the optimal localisation is also lacking. With respect to the frequency given, we classified into high (>1 Hz) and low (≤1 Hz) frequency, according to customary practice. Although localisation, frequency and treatment duration were the main variations, other potential differences in the administration of rTMS that we did not categorise include shape of the coil, number of trains per session and the duration of each train.

Data analysis considerations

In eight of the twelve studies included in the meta-analysis for the HRSD and in six of the seven included in the meta-analysis for the BDI, the baseline mean values for the severity of depression were higher in the treatment group than in the placebo group. Although these differences were not statistically significant at the level of each individual study, they would have introduced a potential bias within the meta-analysis of pooled data by accentuating the tendency for regression to the mean of the more extreme values (Reference DavisDavis, 1976). Our study was limited because individual patient data were not available from all the studies and an appropriate adjustment according to baseline severity was not possible. In order to reduce, as much as possible, any potential bias caused by these differences in baseline values, we compared final values on depression severity between active and sham groups.

Before our study, a meta-analysis (Reference McNamara, Ray and ArthursMcNamara et al, 2001) that included five studies found demonstrable beneficial effects of rTMS in depression. Our findings differ from this earlier paper with respect to the main unit of analysis: this earlier study used the difference in an undefined rate of improvement between groups from psychometric scales used in the trials. In our meta-analysis we used the means and standard deviations because we considered that, owing to probable baseline imbalance between the studies, these estimates reflect a more precise effect size than a dichotomous measure such as the rate of improvement apparently derived from the continuous data of the rating scales.

Consequences of the weak findings about rTMS

Repetitive TMS is a relatively affordable method of applying magnetic fields non-invasively to the human brain. If safety precautions are followed, it also appears to be safe, at least when given within the parameters studied so far: between 1 and 4 weeks of treatment. The non-invasive nature of the intervention has been among the factors that have led to the impetus to research possible therapeutic effects in the treatment of depression and especially in refractory depression, because few other (and certainly no non-invasive) treatment options are currently available. The results of our systematic review show that results to date are not very encouraging.

But this should not be a reason to abandon rTMS in affective disorders altogether. Many of the clinical treatments now used successfully in psychiatry have developed slowly, going through a process of initial enthusiastic approval followed by almost total demise and then back to sensible, widespread clinical use. Electroconvulsive therapy–another method of brain stimulation in affective disorders–underwent this very process. Evidence shows that rTMS has effects on the brain and it therefore has great potential as a research tool (Reference HallettHallett, 2000; Reference Lisanby, Luber and PereraLisanby et al, 2000). Data from animal studies demonstrate effects on expression of immediate early genes (Reference Ji, Schlaepfer and AizenmanJi et al, 1998) and on neuroendocrinology (Reference Keck, Sillaber and EbnerKeck et al, 2000), and rTMS either alone or combined with procedures such as functional neuroimaging (Speer et al, 2000, Reference Speer, Kimbrell and WassermannSpeer et al, 2000) may be useful for testing functional connectivity, neuroplasticity and information processing. Repetitive TMS therefore can be used to test either general hypotheses concerning brain function at different levels or hypotheses concerning the underlying pathology of affective and other neuropsychiatric disorders. It is worthy of note that one of the only two studies included in the meta-analysis in which all the patients were free of medication before and during the rTMS trial was also the only individual study that showed a statistically significant positive effect on the HRSD for the intervention group.

Today, the total number of patients included in studies of the efficacy of rTMS in the treatment of depression falls far short of the numbers registered in trials for new drug treatments. In addition, many technical details, such as where to stimulate, at what frequency, the total number of stimuli and the duration of the treatment, have yet to be resolved. There is an urgent need for thorough, randomised, controlled, multi-centre studies involving large numbers of patients. Another problem is the lack of consensus about the possible explanatory mechanisms for any anti-depressant effects of TMS, but this is also the case for many other treatments in psychiatry. Repetitive TMS research is basically empirical: many variables play a role and a large number of parameters has to be explored carefully to find the most efficacious treatment.

Repetitive TMS clearly has effects on the brain, an observation that is remarkable in itself and it may well be that it is a treatment modality in search of a suitable application in psychiatry. It is of utmost importance, therefore, that the long and difficult path of research for potential clinical applications of rTMS in affective disorders should continue.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. The findings from this systematic review and meta-analysis provide insufficient evidence to suggest that repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) is effective in the treatment of depression.

  2. There are several confounding factors in the included studies that should be kept in mind before considering rTMS for clinical use.

  3. The rTMS technique needs more high-quality trials to show its effectiveness for therapeutic use.

LIMITATIONS

  1. Lack of pragmatic variables in the studies, such as time to further treatment, time off work, readmission to hospital or hospital discharge.

  2. Individual patient data from all the studies were not available and an appropriate adjustment according to baseline severity therefore was not possible.

  3. Poor follow-up evaluation in the studies.

Acknowledgements

The reviewers are grateful for the advice and support of: the Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain (grant number 00/10099); Xavier Bonfill and Montse Sacristán from the Iberoamerican Cochrane Centre and the Service of Clinical Epidemiology and Public Health, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau (Barcelona, Spain); Natalie Khin and Hugh McGuire from the Cochrane Collaboration Depression Neurosis and Anxiety Review Group; Alfonso Casado from 3D Health Research (Barcelona, Spain); Julio Sánchez Meca from the Universidad de Murcia (Spain); and all the trialists who shared their data.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

The study was funded by the Ministerio de Sanidad y Consumo, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Spain (grant no. 00/10099). T. E.S. is involved in one of the studies included in this review and is also Secretary-Treasurer of the International Society for Transcranial Stimulation.

References

APPENDIX 1: Studies included in the review

Avery, D. H., Claypoole, K., Robinson, L., et al ( 1999 ) Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of medication-resistant depression: preliminary data. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 87, 114–117.Google Scholar
Berman, R. M., Narasimhan, M., Sanacora, G., et al ( 2000 ) A randomized clinical trial of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 332–337.Google Scholar
Eschweiler, W. G., Wegerer, C., Schlotter, W., et al ( 2000 ) Left prefrontal activation predicts therapeutic effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in major depression. Psychiatry Research–Neuroimaging, 99, 161–172.Google Scholar
García-Toro, M., Mayol, A., Amillas, H., et al ( 2001a ) Modest adjunctive benefit with transcranial magnetic stimulation in medication-resistant depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 64, 271–275.Google Scholar
García-Toro, M., Pascual-Leone, A., Romera, M., et al ( 2001b ) Prefrontal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation as add on treatment in depression. Journal of Neurology and Psychiatry, 71, 546–548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, M. S., Wassermann, E. M., Kimbrell,T. A., et al ( 1997 ) Mood improvement following daily left prefrontal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with depression: a placebo-controlled crossover trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 1752–1756.Google Scholar
George, M.S., Nahas, Z., Molloy, M., et al ( 2000 ) A controlled trial of daily left prefrontal cortex TMS for treating trial of dailyleftprefrontalcortexTMS for depression. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 962–970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimbrell, T. A., Little, J. T., Dunn, R. T., et al ( 1999 ) Frequency dependence of antidepressant response to left prefrontal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) as a function of baseline cerebral glucose metabolism. Biological Psychiatry, 46, 1603–1613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, E., Kreinin, I., Chistyakov, A., et al ( 1999 ) Therapeutic efficacy of right prefrontal slow repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in major depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 315–320.Google Scholar
Loo, C., Mitchell, P., Sachdev, P., et al ( 1999 ) Double-blind controlled investigation of transcranial magnetic stimulation for the treatment of resistant major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 946–948.Google Scholar
Manes, F., Jorge, R., Morcuende, M., et al ( 2001 ) A controlled study of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation as a treatment of depression in the elderly. International Psychogeriatrics, 13, 225–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Padberg, F., Zwanger, P., Thomas, H., et al ( 1999 ) Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in pharmacotherapy-refractory major depression: comparative study of fast, slow and sham rTMS. Psychiatry Research, 88, 163–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szuba, M.P., O'Reardon, J.P., Rai, A. S., Szuba, M. P., O'Reardon, J. P., Rai, A. et al ( 2001 ) Acute mood and thyroid stimulating hormone effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation in major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 22–27.Google Scholar

APPENDIX 2: Studies excluded in the review: Avery–George–Hotzheimer Database of rTMS Depression Studies. .

Bohning, D. E., Shastri, A., McConnell, K. A., et al ( 1999 ) A combined TMS/fMRI study of intensity-dependent TMS over motor cortex. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 385–394.Google Scholar
Clark, L., McTavish, S. F. B., Harmer, C. J., et al ( 2000 ) Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to right prefrontal cortex does not modulate the psychostimulant effects of amphetamine rTMS and amphetamine response. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 3, 297–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conca, A., Koppi, S., König, P., et al ( 1996 ) Transcranial magnetic stimulation: A novel antidepressive strategy? Neuropsychobiology, 34, 204–207.Google Scholar
Conca, A., Swoboda, E., König, P., et al ( 2000 ) Clinical impacts of single transcranial magnetic stimulation (sTMS) as an add-on therapy in severely depressed patients under SSRI treatment. Human Psychopharmacology–Clinical and Experimental, 15, 429–438.Google Scholar
D'Alfonso, A. A. L., Van Honk, J., Hermans, E., et al ( 2000 ) Laterality effects in selective attention to threat after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at the prefrontal cortex in female subjects. Neuroscience Letters, 280, 195–198.Google Scholar
Eichhammer, P., Kharraz, A., Wiegand, R., et al ( 2002 ) Sleep deprivation in depression stabilizing antidepressant effects by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Life Sciences, 70, 1741–1749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, C. M., Figiel, G. S., Mcdonald, W. M., et al ( 1998 ) Rapid rate transcranial magnetic stimulation in young and middle-aged refractory depressed patients. Psychiatric Annals, 28, 36–39.Google Scholar
Feinsod, M., Kreinin, B., Chistyakov, A., et al ( 1998 ) Preliminary evidence for a beneficial effect of low-frequency, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with major depression and schizophrenia. Focus on Depression and Anxiety, 7, 65–68.Google Scholar
Figiel, G. S., Epstein, C., Mcdonald, W. M., et al ( 1998 ) The use of rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in refractory depressed patients. Neuroscience, 10, 20–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García, M., Micó, J., Crespí, M., et al (1998) Estimulación magnética transcraneal repetitiva: una nueva intervención neurobiológica buscando un lugar en psiquiatría. A propósito de cinco casos. Psqiuiatría.COM, http://www.psiquiatria.com/psiquiatria/vol2num2/art_9.htm Google Scholar
Geller, V., Grisaru, N., Abarbanel, J. M., et al ( 1997 ) Slow magnetic stimulation of prefrontal cortex in depression and schizophrenia. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 21, 105–110.Google Scholar
George, M. S., Wassermann, E. M., Williams, W. A., et al ( 1995 ) Daily repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) improves mood in depression. Neuroreport, 6, 1853–1856.Google Scholar
George, M. S., Wassermann, E. M., Williams, W. A., et al ( 1996 ) Changesin mood and hormone levels after rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) of the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 8, 172–180.Google Scholar
George, M. S., Speer, A. M., Molloy, M., et al ( 1998 ) Low frequency daily left prefrontal rTMS improves mood in bipolar depression: a placebo-controlled case report. Human Psychopharmacology, 13, 271–275.Google Scholar
George, M. S., Lisanby, S. H. & Sackeim, H. A. ( 1999a ) Transcranial magnetic stimulation: applications in neuropsychiatry. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 300–311.Google Scholar
George, M. S., Nahas, Z., Kozel, F. A., et al ( 1999b ) Improvement of depression following transcranial magnetic stimulation. Current Psychiatry Reports, 1, 114–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grisaru, N., Chudakov, B.,Yaroslavsky,Y., et al ( 1998 ) Transcranial magnetic stimulation in mania: a controlled study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 1608–1610.Google Scholar
Grunhaus, L., Dannom, P. N., Schreiber, S., et al ( 2000 ) Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation is an effective electroconvulsive therapy in the treatment of nondelusional major depressive disorder: an open study. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 314–324.Google Scholar
Habel, U., Wild, B., Topka, H., et al ( 2001 ) Transcranial magnetic stimulation: no effect on mood with single pulse during learned helplessness. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 25, 497–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, P. E. B. ( 2000 ) Repetitive magnetic stimulation in the treatment of depression. A review [Repetitiv transkranial magnetisk stimulation]. Ugeskrift for Læger, 162, 2310–2313.Google Scholar
Höflich, G., Kasper, S., Hufnagel, A., et al ( 1993 ) Application of transcranial magnetic stimulation in treatment of drug-resistant major depression–a report of two cases. Human Psychopharmacology, 8, 361–365.Google Scholar
Kolbinger, H. M., Höflich, G., Hufnagel, H., et al ( 1995 ) Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in the treatment of major depression–a pilot study. Human Psychopharmacology, 10, 305–310.Google Scholar
Koppi, S., Conca, A., Swoboda, E., et al ( 1997 ) Transcranial magnetic stimulation–new antidepressive therapy? [Transkranielle magnetstimulation–eine neue antidepressive therapie?]. Psychiatria Danubina, 9, 83–88.Google Scholar
Kozel, F. A., Nahas, Z., deBrux, C., et al ( 2000 ) How coil–cortex distance relates to age, motor threshold, and antidepressant response to repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 12, 376–384.Google Scholar
Krystal, A. D.,West, M., Prado, R., et al ( 2000 ) EEG effects of ECT: implications for rTMS. Depression and Anxiety, 12, 157–165.Google Scholar
Little, J. T., Kimbrell, T. A., Wassermann, E. M., et al ( 2000 ) Cognitive effects of 1- and 20-hertz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in depression: preliminary report. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurology, 13, 119–124.Google Scholar
Loo, C. K., Taylor, J. L., Gandevia, S. C., et al ( 2000 ) Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in controlled treatment studies: are some ‘sham’ forms active? Biological Psychiatry, 47, 325–331.Google Scholar
Loo, C. K., Sachdev, P., Elsayed, H., et al ( 2001 ) Effects of 2- to 4-week course of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) on neuropsychologic functioning, electroencephalogram, and auditory threshold in depressed patients. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 615–623.Google Scholar
Markwort, S., Cordes, P. & Aldenhoff, J. ( 1997 ) Transcranial magnetic stimulation as a therapeutic alternative to electroconvulsive therapy in therapy-resistant depressions [Die transkranielle magnet stimulation als behandlungs alternative zur elektrokrampf therapie bei therapieresistenten depressionen]. Fortschritte Neurologie, Psychiatrie und Ihrer Grenzgebiete, 65, 540–549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menkes, D. L., Bodnar, P., Ballesteros, R. A., et al ( 1999 ) Right frontal lobe slow frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (SF r-TMS) is an effective treatment for depression: a case–control pilot study of safety and efficacy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 67, 113–115.Google Scholar
Moser, D. J., Jorge, R. E., Manes, F., et al ( 2002 ) Improved executive functioning following repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology, 58, 1288–1290.Google Scholar
Mosimann, U. P., Rihs, T. A., Engeler, J., et al ( 2000 ) Mood effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of left prefrontal cortex in healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Research, 94, 251–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nahas, Z., Bohning, D. E., Molloy, M. A., et al ( 1999 ) Safety and feasibility of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of anxious depression in pregnancy: a case report. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60, 50–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nahas, Z., DeBrux, C., Chandler, V., et al ( 2000 ) Lack of significant changes on magnetic resonance scans before and after 2 weeks of daily left prefrontal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for depression. Journal of ECT, 16, 380–390.Google Scholar
Padberg, F., Schüle, C., Zwanger, P., et al ( 2002 ) Relation between responses to repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation and partial sleep deprivation in major depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 36, 131–135.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, A., Catalá, M. D., Pascual-Leone, P. ( 1996a ) Lateralized effect of rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation of the prefrontal cortex on mood. Neurology, 46, 499–502.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, A., Rubio, B., Pallardo, F., et al ( 1996b ) Rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation of left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in drug-resistant depression. Lancet, 347, 233–237.Google Scholar
Pridmore, S. ( 1999 ) Rapid transcranial magnetic stimulation and normalization of the dexamethasone suppression test. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 53, 33–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pridmore, S. ( 2000 ) Substitution of rapid transcranial magnetic stimulation treatments for electroconvulsive therapy treatments in a course of electroconvulsive therapy. Depression and Anxiety, 12, 118–123.Google Scholar
Pridmore, S., & Belmaker, R. ( 1999 ) Transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of psychiatric disorders. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 53, 541–548.Google Scholar
Pridmore, S., Rybak, M., Turnier-Shea, Y., et al ( 1999 ) A naturalistic study of response in melancholia to transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). German Journal of Psychiatry, 2, 13–21.Google Scholar
Pridmore, S., Bruno, R., Turnier-Shea, Y., et al ( 2000 ) Comparison of unlimited numbers of rapid transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) and ECT treatment sessions in major depressive episode. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 3, 129–134.Google Scholar
Reid, P.D. & Pridmore, S. ( 1999 ) Dexamethasone suppression test reversal in rapid transcranial magnetic stimulation-treated depression. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 33, 274–277.Google Scholar
Schouten, E. A. M., D'Alfonso, A. A. L., Nolen, W. A., et al ( 1999 ) Small effect of rapid rate transcranial magnetic stimulation on antidepressant free outpatients with a depressive disorder. A pilot study [Beperkte effectiviteit van transcraniële magnetische stimulatie bij depressie Een oriënterend onderzoek]. Tijdschrift Voor Psychiatrie, 4, 233–237.Google Scholar
Shajahan, P. (2000) The treatment of partial remission in depression using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS): open pilot trial followed by randomised placebo controlled cross-over study. National Research Register, Issue 3. http://www.update-software.com/national/.Google Scholar
Speer, A. M., Kimbrell, T. A., Wassermann, E. M., et al ( 2000 ) Opposite effects of high and low frequency rTMS on regional brain activity in depressed patients. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 1133–1141.Google Scholar
Speer, A. M., Repella, J. D., Figueras, S., et al ( 2001 ) Lack of adverse cognitive effects of 1 Hz and 20 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation at 100% of motor threshold over left prefrontal cortex in depression. Journal of ECT, 17, 259–263.Google Scholar
Stikhina, N. Y. A., Lyskov, E. B., Lomarev, M. P., et al ( 1999 ) Transcranial magnetic stimulation in neurotic depression [Transkranial'naia magnitnaia stimuliatsiia pri nevroticheskoi depressii]. Zhurnal Nevrologii i Psikhiatrii Imeni SS Korsakova, 99, 26–29.Google Scholar
Szuba, M. P., O'Reardon, J. P. & Evans, D. L. ( 2000 ) Physiological effects of electroconvulsive therapy and transcranial magnetic stimulation in major depression. Depression and Anxiety, 12, 170–177.Google Scholar
Teneback, C., Nahas, Z., Speer, A. M., et al ( 1999 ) Changes in prefrontal cortex and paralimbic activity in depression following two weeks of daily left prefrontal TMS. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 11, 426–435.Google Scholar
Tormos, J. M., Catalá, M. D. & Pascual-Leone, A. ( 1999 ) Transcranial magnetic stimulation [Estimulación magnética transcraneal]. Revista de Neurología, 29, 165–171.Google Scholar
Triggs, W. J., McCoy, K. J. M., Greer, R., et al ( 1999 ) Effects of left frontal transcranial magnetic stimulation on depressed mood, cognition, and corticomotor threshold. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 1440–1446.Google Scholar
Turnier-Shea, Y., Rybak, M., Reid, P., et al ( 1999 ) Update on psychotropic medication used concurrently with transcranial magnetic stimulation. German Journal of Psychiatry, 2, 46–59.Google Scholar
Walter, G., Tormos, J. M., Israel, J. A., et al ( 2001 ) Transcranial magnetic stimulation in young persons: a review of known cases. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 11, 69–75.Google Scholar
Woodruff, P. (2000) Cost effectiveness of transcranial magnetic stimulation and electroconvulsive treatment in severe depression (TRANSECT). National Research Register, Issue 3. http://www.update-software.com/national/.Google Scholar
Zheng, X. M. ( 2000 ) Regional cerebral blood flow changes in drug-resistant depressed patients following treatment with transcranial magnetic stimulation: a statistical parametric mapping analysis. Psychiatry Research, 100, 75–80.Google Scholar

References

American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM–IV). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., et al (1961) An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1, 5363.Google Scholar
Berger, V. W. & Exner, D. V. (1999) Detecting selection bias in randomized clinical trials. Control Clinical Trials, 20, 319327.Google Scholar
Clarke, M. & Oxman, A. D. (2001) Cochrane Reviewers Handbook 4.1.3 (update June 2001). Cochrane Library, issue 3. Oxford: Update Software.Google Scholar
Cochrane Collaboration (2000) Review Manager (Revman), Version 4.1 for Windows. Oxford: The Cochrane Collaboration.Google Scholar
Colton, T. (1974) Statistics in Medicine. Boston, MA: Little, Brown and Company.Google Scholar
Davis, C. E. (1976) The effect of regression to the mean in epidemiologic and clinical studies. American Journal of Epidemiology, 104, 493498.Google Scholar
Day, S. J. (2000) Blindingin clinical trials and other studies. BMJ, 321, 504.Google Scholar
Geddes, J. R., Freemantle, N., Mason, J., et al (2002) Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) for depression. Cochrane Library, issue 3. Oxford: Update Software.Google Scholar
George, M. S., Lisonby, S. H. & Sackeim, H. A. (1999) Transcranial magnetic stimulations in neuropsychiatry. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 300311.Google Scholar
Hallett, M. (2000) Transcranial magnetic stimulation and the brain. Nature, 406, 147150.Google Scholar
Hamilton, M. (1960) A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 23, 5662.Google Scholar
Hamilton, M. (1967) Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness. British Journal of the Society of Clinical Psychology, 6, 278296.Google Scholar
Hotopf, M., Churchill, R. & Lewis, G. (1999) Pragmatic randomised controlled trials in psychiatry. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 217223 Google Scholar
Jadad, A. R. (1998) Randomised Controlled Trials. London: BMJ Books.Google Scholar
Ji, R. R., Schlaepfer, T. E., Aizenman, C. D., et al (1998) Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation activates specific regions in rat brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95, 1563515640.Google Scholar
Keck, M. E., Sillaber, I., Ebner, K., et al (2000) Acute transcranial magnetic stimulation of frontal brain regions selectively modulates the release of vasopressin, biogenic amines and amino acids in the rat brain. European Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 37133720.Google Scholar
Lisanby, S. H., Luber, B., Perera, T., et al (2000) Transcranial magnetic stimulation: applications in basic neuroscience and neuropsychopharmacology. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 3, 259273.Google Scholar
Martin, J. L. R. & Casado Collado, A. (2002) Doubleblind. The bias control in the performance of clinical trials. Contradictions, limitations and implications [Doble ciego. El control de los sesgos en la realizaciónde ensayos clínicos. Contradicciones, insuficiencias e implicaciones]. Medicina Clínica, 118, 192195.Google Scholar
Martin, J. L. R., Barbanoj, M. J., Schlaepfer, T. E., et al (2002) Transcranial magnetic stimulation for treating depression. Cochrane Library, issue 2. Oxford: Update Software.Google Scholar
McNamara, B., Ray, J. L., Arthurs, O. J., et al (2001) Transcranial magnetic stimulation for depression and other psychiatric disorders. Psychological Medicine, 31, 11411146.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, R. (2000) Outcome measures of antidepressive therapy. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 101, 4144.Google Scholar
Schulz, K. F., Chalmers, I., Hayes, R. J., et al (1995) Empirical evidence of bias: dimensions of methodological quality associated with estimates of treatment effects in controlled trials. Journal of the American Medical Association, 273, 408412.Google Scholar
Speer, M. M., Kimbrell, T. A., Wassermann, E. M., et al (2000) Opposite effects of high and low frequency rTMS on regional brain activity in depressed patients. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 11331141.Google Scholar
Sutton, A. J., Abrams, K. R., Jones, D. R., et al (2000) Methods for Meta-analysis in Medical Research. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Wassermann, E. M. & Lisanby, S. H. (2001) Therapeutic application of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: a review. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, 13671377.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1993) The ICD–10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Process of inclusion of studies for review and analysis: rTMS, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation; HRSD, Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; ECT, electroconvulsive therapy; mt, motor threshold; *some studies of 2 weeks' duration measured outcomes at both 1 and 2 weeks.

Figure 1

Table 1 Summary of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) placebo-controlled trials

Figure 2

Table 2 Patients' medication regimens during the studies

Figure 3

Fig. 2 Size of effect (remission of symptoms), in the fixed-effect model, of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS, left localisation and high frequency), compared with sham rTMS for depression on the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; subgroup analyses by period of time. SMD, standardised mean difference. 1. Further details available from the authors upon request.

Figure 4

Fig. 3 Size of effect (remission of symptoms), in the fixed-effect model, of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS, left localisation and high frequency) compared with sham rTMS for depression on the Beck Depression Inventory; subgroup analyses by period of time. SMD, standardised mean difference. 1. Further details available from the authors upon request.

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.