Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T04:08:06.092Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Evidence in cannabis research

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

P. Miller*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Arts, Deakin University and Turning Point. Alcohol and Drug Centre, 54–62 Gertrude Street, Fitzroy Victoria 3065, Australia. E-mail: millerp@pipeline.com.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © 2004 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

The article by Coffey et al (Reference Coffey, Carlin and Lynskey2003) regarding adolescent precursors of cannabis dependence has a number of substantial problems in the measures used, the analysis of data and the reporting and discussion of their findings. One of the study's major findings is that the ‘relationship between cannabis dependence and persistent frequent drinking in adolescence changed direction, from a risk association in the univariate model to a protective association in the adjusted model’ (Reference Coffey, Carlin and LynskeyCoffey et al, 2003: p. 333, emphasis added). The use of the term protective implies causality, rather than the negative correlation which more accurately portrays the statistical relationship. It also tacitly implies a value judgement that heavy drinking is preferable to cannabis dependence.

This study utilises logistic regression for the majority of its statistical analysis without adequately considering some important caveats. First and foremost, as already mentioned, correlation does not equal causality. This is particularly the case when there are a substantial number of independent variables associated with the dependent variable. In the case of cannabis use, as the authors point out, there are many independent variables related to cannabis use, such as socio-economic status (not discussed), parental drug use patterns (not discussed), antisocial behaviour, cigarette smoking and level of education, to name a few that are known. Statistical texts (e.g. Reference Gravetter and WallnauGravetter & Wallnau, 1996) point out that to gain the best measure from the use of logistic regression, there should be few independent variables that are unrelated to each other and that ‘a regression solution is extremely sensitive to the combination of variables that is included in it’ (Reference Tabachnick and FidellTabachnick & Fidell, 1996: p. 126).

These issues are particularly concerning when such papers can be reported in the mass media (as this study was) on a topic such as cannabis use, which generates strong public responses and is the forum for a great deal of misinformation and manipulation of research results to suit political and ideological agendas. The simple acknowledgement of study limitations would substantially improve the quality of the debate surrounding such a complex social, psychological and medical problem.

References

Coffey, C., Carlin, J. B., Lynskey, M., et al (2003) Adolescent precursors of cannabis dependence: findings from the Victorian Adolescent Health Cohort Study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 330336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gravetter, F. J. & Wallnau, L. B. (1996) Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences: A First Course for Students of Psychology and Education (4th edn). Minneapolis, MN: West.Google Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. (1996) Using Multivariate Statistics (3rd edn). New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.