Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-p566r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T13:07:24.852Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effect of web-based depression literacy and cognitive–behavioural therapy interventions on stigmatising attitudes to depression

Randomised controlled trial

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Kathleen M. Griffiths*
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health Research, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia
Helen Christensen
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health Research, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia
Anthony F. Jorm
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health Research, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia
Kimberley Evans
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health Research, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia
Chloe Groves
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health Research, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia
*
Dr Kathy Griffiths, Director, Depression and Anxiety Consumer Research Unit, Centre for Mental Health Research, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia. Fax: +62 2 6125 0733; e-mail: Kathy.griffiths@anu.edu.au
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Little is known about the efficacy of educational interventions for reducing the stigma associated with depression.

Aims

To investigate the effects on stigma of two internet depression sites.

Method

A sample of 525 individuals with elevated scores on a depression assessment scale were randomly allocated to a depression information website (BluePages), a cognitive–behavioural skills training website (MoodGYM) or an attention control condition. Personal stigma (personal stigmatising attitudes to depression) and perceived stigma (perception of what most other people believe) were assessed before and after the intervention.

Results

Relative to the control, the internet sites significantly reduced personal stigma, although the effects were small. BluePages had no effect on perceived stigma and MoodGYM was associated with an increase in perceived stigma relative to the control. Changes in stigma were not mediated by changes in depression, depression literacy or cognitive–behavioural therapy literacy.

Conclusions

The internet warrants further investigation as a means of delivering stigma reduction programmes for depression.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 2004 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

The stigma associated with mental illness can adversely affect help-seeking (Reference Wells, Robins and BushnellWells et al, 1994), employment, accommodation (Reference Corrigan, Edwards and GreenCorrigan et al, 2001a ) and mental health (Reference Link, Struening and RahavLink et al, 1997). Evidence-based programmes for reducing stigma are therefore vital. Interpersonal contact with people with mental illness has been reported to be effective in influencing stigma change (Reference Alexander and LinkAlexander & Link, 2003; Reference Couture and PennCouture & Penn, 2003). A number of educational interventions have also been reported as effective in reducing stigma. However, few have focused specifically on depression stigma, and only one of these used a randomised controlled trial design (Reference Corrigan, River and LundinCorrigan et al, 2001b ); most targeted the public or health professionals without mental illness. People with mental disorders also hold stigmatising views about mental illness (Reference Hayward and BrightHayward & Bright, 1997; Reference Ryan, Robinson and HausmannRyan et al, 2001; Reference Corrigan and WatsonCorrigan & Watson, 2002), and anti-stigma programmes should therefore also target those at high risk of developing depression. This paper reports the results of a randomised controlled trial of the effects of web-based depression literacy and cognitive–behavioural interventions on personal and perceived stigma among people who screened positive for depression.

METHOD

Data for this study were collected as part of a larger study of the impact of two internet websites concerned with depression on a range of mental health and other outcomes. The effect of the interventions on depressive symptoms and attitudes to treatment has been reported by Christensen et al, (Reference Christensen, Griffiths and Jorm2004); their paper also contains a CONSORT diagram of the study.

Participants

Participants were 525 adults with elevated scores on a depressive symptom assessment scale. They were recruited by means of a postal screening questionnaire sent to 27000 individuals aged 18–52 years who had been randomly selected from the Canberra electoral roll using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) SELECT CASES random selection option. A total of 6130 people (22.7% response rate; 24.6% excluding those whose questionnaire was returned to sender) completed the screening questionnaire; their mean age was 37.3 years (s.d.=9.9) and 35.8% were male. Respondents were not eligible for inclusion in the trial if they

  1. (a) scored below 12 on the 10-item Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K10; Reference Andrews and SladeAndrews & Slade, 2001) (74.1% of the screening questionnaire respondents);

  2. (b) indicated that they did not have internet access at home or at work (11.4%);

  3. (c) indicated that they did not wish to participate in an intervention (52.1%).

Of the 822 respondents who were potentially eligible based on the screening survey, 24 (2.9%) were found not to have suitable internet access during the recruitment call. In addition, 41 (5%) reported that they were receiving psychiatric or psychological care and were excluded from the trial to ensure that it did not interfere with their treatment. Of the remaining respondents, 657 indicated a willingness to participate in the study and were sent consent and pre-intervention forms. Of these, 525 participants returned completed forms and were randomised to the interventions. The sample comprised 150 men and 375 women. The mean respondent age was 36.4 years (s.d.=9.4) and the mean K10 score was 17.8 (s.d.=5.2).

Design

Eligible individuals who completed consent forms were randomly assigned to access a depression literacy website (BluePages, n=165), a cognitive–behavioural therapy skills training site (MoodGYM, n=182) or an attention control condition (control, n=178). Participants were randomised to interventions by a statistician using the SPSS SELECT CASES random selection option and enrolled in the trial by the project manager. Those enrolled completed pre-intervention surveys by post and began the intervention an average of 1.7 weeks (s.d.=1) later. Screening and interventions were staggered between August 2002 and May 2003. The project protocol was approved by the human ethics committee of the Australian National University.

Interventions

The BluePages website (http://www.bluepages.anu.edu.au) provided information about depression, including symptoms, general and specific sources of help, evidence-based information about medical, psychological and alternative/lifestyle treatments for depression, and information about depression prevention. The site indicated that depression is an illness, provided information that moderate depression is as disabling as multiple sclerosis, emphasised that depression can and should be treated and stressed the importance and wisdom of seeking help. It also included short biographies of famous people who have experienced depression. Descriptions of the rationale for many of the treatments incorporated biomedical explanations of depression (e.g. neurotransmitter depletion), but some emphasised non-biomedical factors or approaches to depression (e.g. conflict reduction in interpersonal therapy, changing negative thoughts and improving social skills in cognitive–behavioural therapy). The intervention consisted of directing participants to read a different specified section of the BluePages site each week for 5 weeks.

The MoodGYM intervention (http://www.moodgym.anu.edu.au) comprised five interactive modules that covered cognitive therapy, behavioural methods for over-coming dysfunctional thinking, relaxation, problem solving, assertiveness and self-esteem training, and strategies for coping with relationship break-up. The intervention did not emphasise biomedical factors but rather focused on training the participant to develop strategies for coping with events and circumstances that might activate depression. As with the BluePages site, participants were directed to read a different specified module of the MoodGYM site each week for 5 weeks.

The control intervention involved weekly contact with an interviewer, who asked open-ended questions about factors that might influence depression, including physical and artistic activities, hobbies and educational activities, relationships, social support and financial management, work stress and relationships, health and nutrition and alcohol intake. The contacts were made at weekly intervals over 5 weeks and were designed to last for approximately 10 min.

Procedures

Individuals who indicated in their screening questionnaire that they might be willing to participate in an intervention trial were contacted by telephone by a trained interviewer, who described each of the three interventions and asked if the respondent would be willing to be randomised to one of them. Participants who agreed were advised that they would soon receive a consent form and pre-intervention questionnaire through the post.

Participants randomised to the two website conditions were provided with a log-in identification number and a manual containing information about the relevant website. This booklet also outlined the sections or modules of the websites that were to be completed for each of the 5 weeks. At the end of the intervention period, participants were sent a post-intervention questionnaire. Lay interviewers were not given access to questionnaire results.

Interviewers maintained weekly telephone contact with participants over the period of the intervention (a total of six contacts of approximately 10 min each: one at the outset of the intervention and one at the end of each of the 5 weeks of the intervention). During these contacts, participants using the websites were asked standard questions about their use of the website over the past week and reminded of their task for the following week. Interviewers were provided with separate instruction booklets for each participant, containing verbatim instructions for each of the weekly contacts. These booklets also included a form for recording telephone calls to participants.

Prior to the study, all interviewers attended a 1-day training session on the trial. Of the six interviewers, none had formal qualifications in mental health or a related field, although four had many years of experience in conducting survey interviews for the Centre for Mental Health Research; the other two were undergraduates without prior interviewing experience.

Measures

Demographic and clinical history, psychological distress, stigma, level of depressive symptoms and mental health knowledge were assessed by postal questionnaire.

Demographic and clinical history

Age, gender, education and previous history of depression were recorded.

Kessler Psychological Distress Scale

This 10-item test of psychological distress, which has been validated on an Australian population (Reference Andrews and SladeAndrews & Slade, 2001), was used to screen for depressive disorder. The scale is scored from 0 to 40, with higher scores indicating greater psychological stress.

Depression stigma scale

Changes in stigma were assessed using an 18-item test that we constructed for the purpose (Table 1; further details available from the authors upon request). Items were derived from major recurring broad themes extracted from websites on depression and stigma. In particular, themes were based on the first 100 results returned using the Google search engine and the keywords STIGMA DEPRESSION and on the Google depression directory using the keyword STIGMA. (Google was selected for the search because of its extensive coverage of the web and its demonstrated superiority in returning relevant content: Reference Hawking, Craswell and BaileyHawking et al, 2001). Themes reflected in the items included status of depression as an illness (‘depression is not a real medical illness’), extent to which depression is under personal control/fault (‘people could snap out of depression if they wanted’), character flaw (‘sign of weakness’), dangerousness, unpredictability, shame/concealment (‘would not tell anyone’), avoidance (‘avoid people with depression’) and discrimination (‘not vote for politician with depression’, ‘not employ someone with depression’). Half of the items required the participant to rate how strongly they personally agreed with a statement about depression (e.g. ‘people with depression are unpredictable’). The other half of the items asked the participant to indicate what they thought most other people believed about the same issue (e.g. ‘most people believe that people with depression are unpredictable’). Ratings were made on a five-point Likert scale. A principal components analysis on the data of the trial participants showed that the scale had two factors:

Table 1 Item-total correlations for the personal and perceived depression stigma scales for the trial group prior to randomisation (n=525)

Item Personal stigma Correlation1 Item Perceived stigma Correlation1
1 People with depression could snap out of it if they wanted 0.40 10 Most people believe that people with depression could snap out of it if they wanted 0.58
2 Depression is a sign of personal weakness 0.47 11 Most people believe that depression is a sign of personal weakness 0.62
3 Depression is not a real medical illness 0.50 12 Most people believe that depression is not a real medical illness 0.63
4 People with depression are dangerous 0.44 13 Most people believe that people with depression are dangerous 0.52
5 It is best to avoid people with depression so you don't become depressed yourself 0.48 14 Most people believe that it is best to avoid people with depression so you don't become depressed yourself 0.49
6 People with depression are unpredictable 0.29 15 Most people believe that people with depression are unpredictable 0.45
7 If I had depression I would not tell anyone 0.29 16 If they had depression, most people would not tell anyone 0.38
8 I would not employ someone if I knew they had been depressed 0.59 17 Most people would not employ someone they knew had been depressed 0.55
9 I would not vote for a politician if I knew they had been depressed 0.56 18 Most people would not vote for a politician they knew had been depressed 0.51

  1. (a) personal stigma (reflecting the participants’ personal attitudes, and comprising nine items);

  2. (b) perceived stigma (reflecting the participants’ beliefs about the attitudes of others, and also comprising nine items).

Scores on the total scale can range from 0 to 36 for the full scale and 0 to 18 for each of the two nine-item sub-scales, with higher scores indicating greater stigma. Cronbach's α values for the total, personal and perceived depression stigma scales were 0.78, 0.76 and 0.82 respectively and the correlation between the scores on the personal and perceived stigma scales was 0.10 (n=525; P=0.02). Individual item total correlations are shown in Table 1.

Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale

Severity of depression was assessed using the 20-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale (CES–D; Reference RadloffRadloff, 1977). Scores on the CES–D range from 0 to 60; higher scores represent greater psychological distress, and a score in excess of 15 was treated as reflecting a clinical level of depression.

Depression Literacy and Cognitive Behaviour Therapy Literacy

Depression literacy was assessed using a 22-item true/false test of knowledge about depression (D–Lit), and cognitive–behavioural therapy literacy was assessed using a 16-item true/false test of knowledge about the principles of this therapy (CBT–Lit). For each of these tests, which were developed for the study, a higher score indicated greater literacy.

Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire

Dysfunctional thoughts were evaluated using the 30-item Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ; Reference Hollon and KendallHollon & Kendall, 1980). Scores on the ATQ range from 30 to 150, with higher scores indicating more severely dysfunctional thoughts.

Analysis

Baseline characteristics of the three groups were compared using one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) for continuous measures and chi-squared analyses for categorical variables. The effects of the interventions were assessed using both intent-to-treat and completer analyses. In the intent-to-treat analyses, participants who did not respond to the post-intervention questionnaire were allocated their baseline score for the post-test score. Personal and perceived stigma were analysed separately using repeated-measures ANOVAs. Where a significant intervention × time interaction effect was obtained, the effect of the intervention on stigma was compared between the conditions using one-way ANOVAs of the change in stigma score (post-intervention score minus pre-intervention score) followed by Bonferroni-adjusted multiple comparisons. All effects were tested at the P < 0.05 level. Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen's d (standardised mean difference; Reference CohenCohen, 1988).

Potential mediators of the intervention programmes were tested using techniques and criteria described by Baron & Kenny (Reference Baron and Kenny1986) and MacKinnon (Reference Mackinnon1994). This approach involves the following steps:

  1. (a) testing whether the programme affects the outcome of interest (i.e. whether the website effects the change in stigma) (Conclusion 1);

  2. (b) testing whether the programme affects the potential mediator or mediators (i.e. whether the website effects change in depression, change in depression literacy, change in cognitive–behavioural therapy literacy, change in dysfunctional thoughts) (Conclusion 2);

  3. (c) testing whether the potential mediators affect the outcome controlling for exposure to the intervention (i.e. whether change in depression literacy, change in cognitive–behavioural therapy literacy or change in dysfunctional thoughts is associated with a change in stigma controlling for exposure to the website) (Conclusion 3);

  4. (d) if these conclusions are satisfied, a test is conducted to determine whether the mediated effect is statistically significant, using the Sobel test (Reference Sobel and LeinhartSobel, 1982) (Conclusion 4).

Analyses were conducted separately for the BluePages and MoodGYM interventions for those who completed the intervention. In addition, analyses were made using both single mediator models (in which the effect of a potential mediator might have been confounded by the contribution of other potential mediators) and multiple mediator models (where the effect of each potential mediator was computed independently of the effect of other potential mediators).

RESULTS

There was no significant difference in participant age for the three conditions: BluePages mean age 37.3 years (s.d.=9.4), MoodGYM mean age 35.9 years (s.d.=9.5), control mean age 36.3 years (s.d.=9.3); F (2,521)=1.01, P=0.37, n=524. The percentages of women allocated to each condition did not differ significantly (BluePages 69%, MoodGYM 75%, control 70%; χ2(2)=1.48, P=0.48). However, there was a significant difference in the number of years of education across the conditions (BluePages mean 15.3 years (s.d.=2.2), MoodGYM mean 15.0 years (s.d.=2.3), control mean 14.7 years (s.d.=2.1); F (2,507)=3.02, P=0.05, n=510), with the control group being slightly less educated than the website intervention groups. Mean K10 scores were similar for the three groups (BluePages mean 17.5 (s.d.=4.9), MoodGYM mean 17.9 (s.d.=5.0), control mean 18.0 (s.d.=5.7); F (2,522)=0.42, P=0.66), as were the mean CES–D scores (BluePages mean 21.1 (s.d.=10.4), MoodGYM mean 21.8 (s.d.=10.5), control mean 21.6 (s.d.=11.1); F (2,522)=0.22, P=0.80). A previous experience with depression was reported by 92%, 95% and 93% of the BluePages, MoodGYM and control groups, respectively) (χ2(2)=1.39, P=0.50, n=524). Baseline depression literacy scores did not differ across groups (BluePages mean 13.2 (s.d.=3.1), MoodGYM mean 12.9 (s.d.=3.4), control mean 13.2 (s.d.=3.6); F (2,522)=0.45, P=0.64). There was no significant effect of condition on baseline personal or perceived stigma (personal stigma, F (2,522)=2.13, P=0.12; perceived stigma, F (2,522)=0.66, P=0.52; Table 2).

Table 2 Personal and perceived stigma scores for each intervention group over time

Personal stigma Perceived stigma
BluePages MoodGYM Control BluePages MoodGYM Control
Intent-to-treat (n=525)
    Group size, n 165 182 178 165 182 178
    Pre-intervention score: mean (s.d.) 9.8 (5.0) 10.7 (5.0) 9.7 (5.0) 23.4 (4.8) 23.5 (4.9) 24.0 (4.7)
    Post-intervention score: mean (s.d.) 9.2 (4.4) 10.2 (5.1) 10.1 (4.8) 23.4 (4.3) 23.9 (4.9) 23.3 (4.7)
Completed the trial (n=414)
    Group size, n 136 121 157 136 121 157
    Pre-intervention score: mean (s.d.) 9.9 (5.0) 10.8 (5.1) 9.4 (5.0) 23.4 (5.0) 23.1 (5.1) 24.0 (4.7)
    Post-intervention score: mean (s.d.) 9.3 (4.3) 10.3 (5.1) 9.8 (4.7) 23.3 (4.4) 23.8 (5.0) 23.3 (4.7)

Participation

Of the 525 participants, 435 (83%) completed the post-intervention survey and 414 (79%) completed both the allocated intervention and the post-intervention survey (Table 2). Participants who returned their questionnaires did not differ in age (F (1,522)=2.68, P=0.10), gender (χ2(1)=1.84, P=0.18) or years of education (F (1,508)=1.81, P=0.18) from those who failed to return their questionnaire. Nor did they differ in baseline personal or perceived stigma scores (personal, F (1,523)= 0.59, P=0.44; perceived, F (1,523)=3.35, P=0.07), although there was a trend towards higher perceived stigma among those who did not complete the questionnaire. The latter group also had higher psychological distress and depression scores at baseline (K10, F (1,523)=7.64, P=0.006; CES–D, F (1,523)=5.15, P=0.024). Questionnaire return rates differed across the intervention groups (χ2(2)=14.18, P < 0.001); in particular, a significantly greater proportion of participants enrolled in the MoodGYM intervention failed to complete the post-intervention survey compared with the BluePages (χ2=5.5, P=0.02) or control (χ2=12.97, P < 0.001) conditions.

Test-retest reliability of depression stigma sub-scales

The test–retest reliabilities based on pre- and post-test data for the combined, control, BluePages and MoodGYM conditions were 0.71 (n=435), 0.66 (n=159), 0.79 (n=136) and 0.70 (n=140) respectively for the personal stigma scale, and 0.67 (n=0.434), 0.67 (n=159), 0.63 (n=135) and 0.73 (n=140) respectively for the perceived stigma scale (P < 0.001 in each case).

Effects for personal stigma

Table 2 shows the mean personal stigma scores for participants who were randomised to an intervention condition (intent-to-treat). The main effect for time was not significant (F (1,522)=2.95, P > 0.05), but there was a significant interaction between time and intervention group (F (2,522)=4.36, P=0.016), indicating that the interventions had different effects on stigma. An analysis of the change in stigma over time demonstrated a significant main effect for intervention (F (2,522)=4.36, P=0.013). In particular, stigma reduction was significantly greater in both the BluePages and MoodGYM groups than in the control condition after Bonferroni correction: BluePages v. control groups, mean difference 0.94, 95% CI 0.07–1.82, P=0.031; MoodGYM v. control groups, mean difference 0.90, 95% CI 0.043–1.75, P=0.036. There was no significant difference between the two websites in stigma reduction (mean difference 0.04, 95% CI -0.83 to 0.92, P=1.0). Re-analysis of the results using education as a covariate yielded the same pattern of findings as above, except the greater stigma reduction in the MoodGYM condition relative to the control only approached significance (P=0.054) after Bonferroni correction. The pattern of results for those who completed both the intervention and the post-intervention questionnaire was similar to that for the intent-to-treat groups, but the effect for MoodGYM (which was significant using an unadjusted t-test) was not significant following Bonferroni adjustment (mean difference 0.96, 95% CI -0.12 to 2.04, P=0.10).

Effects for perceived stigma

The mean perceived stigma scores for each intervention before and after the intervention are shown in Table 2. The main effect for time was not significant (F (1,522)=0.3, P > 0.05), but there was a significant interaction between time and intervention group (F (2,522)=4.16, P=0.016), indicating that the interventions had different effects on perceived stigma. There was a significant effect for intervention on change in stigma (F (2,522)=4.16, P=0.016). In particular, stigma increased in the MoodGYM group relative to the control group (MoodGYM v. control, mean difference -1.07, 95% CI -1.96 to -0.177, P=0.012), but there was no other significant difference in stigma reduction for the interventions (BluePages v. control, mean difference 0.60, 95% CI -1.5 to 0.32, P=0.35; BluePages v. MoodGYM, mean difference -0.47, 95% CI -1.38 to 0.44, P=0.64). Re-analysis of the results using education as a covariate yielded the same pattern of results as above, notably a significant interaction between intervention and time and a significantly greater increase in stigma in the MoodGYM group.

Size of the effects

The pre- minus post-effect sizes for personal stigma were 0.12 (BluePages), 0.11 (MoodGYM) and -0.07 (control) for the intent-to-treat group, and 0.13, 0.10 and -0.09 respectively for those who completed the trial. The corresponding pre–post effect sizes for perceived stigma were 0.01, -0.09 and 0.14 for the intent-to-treat group and 0.02, -0.14 and 0.15 for those who completed the trial.

Analyses of mediation effects

The following analyses included change in depressive symptoms, and change in depression and cognitive–behavioural therapy literacy as potential mediators in the model. However, dysfunctional thoughts were excluded from the model since there was a high correlation between the ATQ and CES–D change scores (BluePages r=0.70, MoodGYM r=0.59, control r=0.71).

Personal stigma

Consistent with the results reported above, the mediation analysis demonstrated that the websites each decreased personal stigma relative to the control condition (Conclusion 1 met). In addition, each of the websites significantly influenced the three potential mediators, being associated with depression reduction, increased depression literacy and increased cognitive–behavioural therapy literacy. For the BluePages group (n=136), ΔCES–D (pre–post) 3.62, P=0.001; Δ D–Lit=-4.28, P < 0.001; ΔCBT–Lit=-1.47, P < 0.001. For the MoodGYM group (n=121), Δ CES–D=4.52, P < 0.001; ΔD–Lit=-0.70, P=0.02; ΔCBT–Lit=-2.84, P < 0.001 (Conclusion 2 met). However, controlling for intervention, the potential mediators did not affect personal stigma levels for either the BluePages or the MoodGYM interventions. This was the case when the effect of a potential mediator was considered controlling for the effects of the other mediators (multiple mediator model) and also when the mediators were considered singly (single mediator model). For the BluePages group (n=136): multiple model, Δ CES–D, β=0.03 (s.e.=0.02), P=0.20; Δ D–Lit=-0.06 (s.e.=0.09), P=0.51; Δ CBT–Lit=-0.04 (s.e.=0.12), P=0.76; single model, Δ CES–D, β=0.03 (s.e.=0.02), P=0.18; Δ D–Lit=-0.07 (s.e.=0.09), P=0.42; Δ CBT–Lit=-0.06 (s.e.=0.12), P=0.64. For the MoodGYM group (n=121): multiple model, ΔCES–D, β=0.02 (s.e.=0.024), P=0.53; ΔD–Lit=-0.08 (s.e.=0.09), P=0.38; ΔCBT–Lit=-0.16 (s.e=0.11), P=0.14; single model, ΔCES–D, β=0.02 (s.e.=0.024), P=0.44; Δ D–Lit=-0.11 (s.e.=0.09), P=0.24; Δ CBT–Lit=-0.19 (s.e.=0.11), P=0.09 (Conclusion 3 not met). Accordingly, it was concluded that the personal stigma reduction effects of the website interventions were not mediated by change in depression or change in depression literacy.

Perceived stigma

Since, as noted above, the BluePages intervention did not lead to a change in perceived stigma (Conclusion 1), no mediation analysis was conducted for this intervention. The MoodGYM intervention, however, was associated with an increase in perceived stigma relative to the control group: β=-1.41 (s.e.=0.49), t=2.87, P=0.004 (Conclusion 1 met). In addition, the relationship between the mediators and change in stigma was significant, as documented above (Conclusion 2 met). Controlling for intervention, greater depression literacy improvement was associated with less perceived stigma reduction for both the single and multiple mediator models: for MoodGYM, multiple model, ΔCES–D, β=0.04 (s.e.=0.03), P=0.14; ΔD–Lit=-0.21 (s.e.=0.10), P=0.04; Δ CBT–Lit=0.04 (s.e.=0.12), P=0.77; single model, Δ CES–D, β=0.04 (s.e.=0.03), P=0.10; Δ D–Lit=-0.22 (s.e.=0.10), P=0.03; Δ CBT–lIT=-0.02 (s.e.=0.12), P=0.89 (Conclusion 3 met for depression literacy change). However, the mediated effect was not significant (ratio of the indirect to the direct effect -0.10, Sobel test 1.65, P=0.10). The other mediators (change in symptoms, depression literacy) did not lead to perceived stigma change (Conclusion 3 not met). In conclusion, there was no evidence that the perceived stigma increase for MoodGYM relative to the control group was associated with change in depression or change in depression or cognitive–behavioural therapy literacy.

DISCUSSION

This randomised controlled trial demonstrated that, relative to an attention control group, both a web-based depression literacy intervention and a web-based cognitive–behavioural intervention resulted in a small but statistically significant reduction in stigmatising attitudes towards depression among people with high levels of depressive symptoms. In contrast, the depression literacy intervention had no effect on participant perception of the extent to which others held stigmatising attitudes. However, the web-based cognitive–behavioural therapy resulted in an increase in perceived stigma relative to the control group. There was no evidence that any of the changes in stigmatising attitudes were mediated by changes in depression, or in depression or cognitive–behavioural therapy literacy.

Although the magnitude of the effect sizes for the web-based interventions are small, particularly when compared with that usually required for a clinical intervention, even small effect sizes can translate into worthwhile gains in a public health context involving large numbers of people and where an intervention can be delivered conveniently and at low cost (Reference Jorm, Griffiths and ChristensenJorm et al, 2003). The MoodGYM website, for example, currently attracts 17000 unique visitors per month.

The finding that an educational intervention can reduce stigmatising attitudes has been reported previously for the conditions of schizophrenia and ‘mental illness’ (e.g. Reference MorrisonMorrison, 1977; Reference Morrison and TetaMorrison & Teta, 1979; Reference Holmes, Corrigan and WilliamsHolmes et al, 1999; Reference Penn, Kommana and MansfieldPenn et al, 1999; Reference Corrigan, River and LundinCorrigan et al, 2001b ; Pinfold et al, Reference Pinfold, Huxley and Thornicroft2003a ,Reference Pinfold, Toulmin and Thornicroft b ), primarily in quasi-experimental or observational studies. However, to our knowledge there has been no previous study of the effect of a psychoeducational intervention on personal stigma among people with elevated depressive symptoms or a history of depression. There has been one study (a randomised controlled trial) of the effects of a psychoeducational programme in reducing the stigma associated with depression among adults enrolled in a community college (Reference Corrigan, River and LundinCorrigan et al, 2001b ). This brief face-to-face programme rebutted a series of myths about mental illness. The researchers found no effect of the programme on negative attributions for controllability in depression (belief that depressed people are to blame for their problems; pre–post effect size 0.04), but the programme did reduce negative stability attributions about depression (belief that depression is treatable; pre–post effect size 0.48) Corrigan et al's programme has the advantage that it requires significantly less time commitment from the participant than does the BluePages website. However, its demonstrated effects for depression were limited to stability attributions. Moreover, the BluePages website has the advantage that its positive effects are not confined to stigma; it is also associated with significant improvement in depressive symptoms and depression literacy among participants (Reference Christensen, Griffiths and JormChristensen et al, 2004).

There was some evidence from our study that recipients of cognitive–behavioural therapy showed a reduction in personal stigma. Significantly, this effect was not mediated by decreased depressive symptoms. To our knowledge, this is the first study to report the impact of cognitive–behavioural therapy on stigma. It has previously been proposed that cognitive–behavioural therapy might be useful in reducing self-stigma (Reference Hayward and BrightHayward & Bright, 1997; Reference GrayGray, 2002). In particular, Hayward & Bright (Reference Hayward and Bright1997) proposed addressing self-stigma by assessing specific self-denigrating beliefs and combating them using a cognitive–behavioural approach. Interestingly, the MoodGYM intervention did not specifically target stigmatising attitudes; thus, any stigma-reducing effect of the intervention might have been due to generalisation of the cognitive techniques to the domain of stigma. Conceivably, a more targeted approach such as that suggested by Hayward & Bright (Reference Hayward and Bright1997) would have been more effective. An alternative explanation for the results is that MoodGYM reinforced the message that psychological problems are amenable to intervention, which in turn was associated with a reduction in personal stigma. Although the results for personal stigma are encouraging, the finding that cognitive–behavioural therapy was associated with an increase in perceived stigma requires some attention. It might have been expected that as the dysfunctional thoughts associated with depression decreased, perceived stigma would also decrease. This was not the case. It is possible that the emphasis in MoodGYM on changing emotions by changing thoughts and behaviours led participants to perceive that others believe that depression is under the control (and hence is the fault) of the depressed person. It has been proposed that such attributions of controllability are associated with stigmatising reactions (Reference Hayward and BrightHayward & Bright, 1997). It is of interest that whatever mechanism led to increased perceived stigma in the MoodGYM group did not influence the personal views of the group. Nevertheless, it might be advisable for clinicians delivering cognitive–behavioural therapy to consider strategies for reducing perceived stigma, particularly if it is associated with the therapy itself.

The finding that the control group showed a decrease in perceived stigma was unexpected but of potential interest. It is possible that part of this effect was due to some active component of the weekly telephone interviews. Whereas interviews with the internet participants focused on programme activities and feedback (e.g. ‘Did you visit any links to other websites this week?’), questions asked of control participants related to their own personal experiences (e.g. to what extent they thought particular activities protected or put them at risk of depression). Perhaps the experience of speaking about these matters to another, non-judgemental person (in this case the trial interviewer) affected participants’ perceptions about the attitudes of others.

Limitations of the study

The major limitation of this study is that the effect sizes were small. There are a number of possible reasons for this. First, pre-intervention personal stigma was not marked, the approximate mean score being 10 (out of a maximum score of 36). This might have placed a floor on the potential for participant improvement. Second, the intervention might have been suboptimal for the current purposes. In particular, neither the exercises nor the other content of MoodGYM specifically addressed issues of self-stigma. Similarly, BluePages was not specifically designed as an anti-stigma package but rather as a site about depression in general, with an emphasis on evidence-based treatments. BluePages did incorporate an illness/biomedical model of depression and the strong message that depression is treatable, with the aim (in part) of reducing controllability and stability attributions, since social attribution theory predicts that reducing these attributions should reduce stigma (Reference Corrigan, River and LundinCorrigan et al, 2000). However, it has been argued that the biomedical model of mental illness might increase stigma (e.g. Reference Walker and ReadWalker & Read, 2002; Reference Dietrich, Beck and BujantugsDietrich et al, 2004). Inclusion of the model in BluePages might therefore have reduced the effect size of the intervention. However, any effects on stigma of a biomedical explanation could operate differently for people with and without depression, and more targeted research on the role of the biomedical model is needed (Reference Griffiths and ChristensenGriffiths & Christensen, 2004).

Another limitation of our study is that stigma measured by means of self-completed questionnaires may lack ecological validity, possibly measuring participant cognitions or hypotheses rather than their attitudes in a real-life context (Reference HaghighatHaghighat, 2001). In addition, apparent changes in self-reported stigma may reflect social desirability bias (Reference HaghighatHaghighat, 2001).

Because our study was conducted in the broader context of a depression intervention trial (Reference Christensen, Griffiths and JormChristensen et al, 2004), the control condition was designed to be credible as an intervention for depression. It did not therefore incorporate a neutral website, a limitation for the purposes of this study because the nature of the interviewer contacts for the control group differed from those for the website groups. Another potential weakness of our findings is that the response rate for the screening survey was low. However, as we have noted in a previous paper, the more relevant factor in a design such as this is the response rate among people with a high level of depressive symptoms (Reference Jorm, Griffiths and ChristensenJorm et al, 2003). Using data from the Australian National Survey of Mental Health and Well-Being (Reference Andrews and SladeAndrews & Slade, 2001), it is estimated that 1831 people in the original sample of 27 000 would have obtained a K10 score in the required range. In fact, 1586 (86.6%) people in this range returned completed surveys. Thus, the response rate to the screening survey was high among the target group of interest. Only 33.1% (525) of respondents with a high level of depressive symptoms met other necessary criteria for inclusion in the trial, including having internet access, not currently receiving treatment from a psychologist or psychiatrist and being willing to participate. It seems likely, however, that the attributes of the intervention sample reflect the subset of the community who would be willing to undertake an internet intervention to decrease psychological distress.

Another limitation of our study is that testing occurred soon after completion of the interventions. Clearly, a meaningful intervention must have longer-term sustainable effects. In addition, since the current personal stigma scale clustered into one factor, it is not possible to determine if the intervention differentially affected different aspects of personal stigma. Moreover, it is not possible to judge from the study which aspects of the websites’ content were effective in reducing personal stigma. Finally, it remains to be seen if BluePages can reduce personal stigma among people without depressive symptoms.

Future research

The internet is used by a range of organisations and individuals to deliver public health messages designed to combat stigmatising attitudes to mental health disorders. However, the effect of these sites on stigma has not previously been evaluated. The results of this study suggest that the internet warrants further investigation as a potentially effective means of delivering stigma reduction programmes for depression. The challenge is to develop programmes that exert larger effects on stigma. This would involve systematically testing and identifying the content and contexts which are most effective in reducing stigma. There would also be value in comparing the efficacy of different delivery methods, including face-to-face interviews, internet websites and printed material. Finally, there is a need to explore the effectiveness of educational programmes in reducing stigma among family members of people with depression, among people without depressive symptoms and among people with high levels of stigma about depression.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. Depression literacy and cognitive–behavioural therapy programmes may be helpful in decreasing personal stigma in people with depression.

  2. The internet offers an opportunity to disseminate such programmes widely and at low cost.

  3. Care should be taken to ensure that cognitive–behavioural programmes do ot increase perceived stigma among clients.

LIMITATIONS

  1. The demonstrated effect sizes were small.

  2. Further research is needed to identify which messages are most effective.

  3. It is not known if the positive effects of these psychoeducational interventions are sustainable over time.

Acknowledgements

We are indebted to the participants in this study. We are grateful to Dr. Keith Dear, statistician, Centre for Mental Health Research (CMHR); Dr David Hawking and his colleagues at the Commonwealth Scientific and Research Organisation for the development of the P@NOPTIC depression search facility; Mr Anthony Bennett (CMHR) for BluePages information technology support; Mr. David Berriman and colleagues from Corporate Information System; Australian National University, for MoodGYM information technology support; Ms Claire Kelly and the CMHR Mental Health Literacy Team; the CMHR lay interviewers; the BluePages advisory board; Ms Jo Medway; and the many other people who assisted in the development of MoodGYM. The study was funded by a National Health and Medical Research Council Program Grant.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

Alexander, L. A. & Link, B. G. (2003) The impact of contact on stigmatizing attitudes toward people with mental illness. Journal of Mental Health, 12, 271289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, G. & Slade, T. (2001) Interpreting scores on the Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (KI0). Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health, 25, 494497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, R. & Kenny, D. (1986) The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, H., Griffiths, K. M. & Jorm, A. F. (2004) Delivering interventions for depression by using the internet: randomised controlled trial. BMJ, 328, 265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd edn). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.Google Scholar
Corrigan, P. W. & Watson, A. C. (2002) The paradox of self-stigma and mental illness. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 9, 3553.Google Scholar
Corrigan, P. W., River, L. P., Lundin, R. K., et al (2000) Stigmatizing attributions about mental illness. Journal of Community Psychology, 28, 91102.Google Scholar
Corrigan, P. W., Edwards, A. B., Green, A., et al (2001a) Prejudice, social distance, and familiarity with mental illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27, 219225.Google Scholar
Corrigan, P. W., River, L. P., Lundin, R. K., et al (2001b) Three strategies for changing attributions about severe mental illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27, 187195.Google Scholar
Couture, S. M. & Penn, D. L. (2003) Interpersonal contact and the stigma of mental illness: a review of the literature. Journal of Mental Health, 12, 291305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dietrich, S., Beck, M., Bujantugs, B., et al (2004) The relationship between public causal beliefs and social distance toward mentally ill people. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 38, 348354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, A. J. (2002) Stigmain psychiatry. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 95, 7276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, K. M. & Christensen, H. (2004) Commentary on ‘The relationship between public causal beliefs and social distance toward mentally ill people'. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 38, 355357.Google Scholar
Haghighat, R. (2001) A unitary theory of stigmatisation: pursuit of self-interest and routes to destigmatisation. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 207215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawking, D. A., Craswell, N. E., Bailey, P. R., et al (2001) Measuring search engine quality. Information Retrieval, 4, 339.Google Scholar
Hayward, P. & Bright, J. A. (1997) Stigma and mental illness: a review and critique. Journal of Mental Health, 6, 345354.Google Scholar
Hollon, S. D. & Kendall, P. C. (1980) Cognitive self statement in depression: development of an Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 383395.Google Scholar
Holmes, E. P., Corrigan, P. W., Williams, P., et al (1999) Changing attitudes about schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25, 447456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, A. F., Griffiths, K. M., Christensen, H., et al (2003) Providing information about the effectiveness of treatment options to depressed people in the community: a randomized controlled trial of effects on mental health literacy, help-seeking and symptoms. Psychological Medicine, 33, 10711079.Google Scholar
Link, B. G., Struening, E. L., Rahav, M., et al (1997) On stigma and its consequences: evidence from a longitudinal study of men with dual diagnoses of mental illness and substance abuse. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 38, 177190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackinnon, D. (1994) Analysis of mediating variables in prevention and intervention research. NIDA Research Monograph, 139, 127153.Google Scholar
Morrison, J. K. (1977) Changing negative attributions to mental patients by means of demythologizing seminars. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33, 549551.3.0.CO;2-P>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, J. K. & Teta, D. C. (1979) Impact of a humanistic approach on students’ attitudes, attributions, and ethical conflicts. Psychological Reports, 45, 863866.Google Scholar
Penn, D. L., Kommana, S., Mansfield, M., et al (1999) Dispelling the stigma of schizophrenia: II. The impact of information on dangerousness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25, 437446.Google Scholar
Pinfold, V., Huxley, P., Thornicroft, G., et al (2003a) Reducing psychiatric stigma and discrimination. Evaluating an educational intervention with the police force in England. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 38, 337344.Google Scholar
Pinfold, V., Toulmin, H., Thornicroft, G., et al (2003b) Reducing psychiatric stigma and discrimination: evaluation of educational interventions in UK secondary schools. British Journal of Health Psychology, 182, 342346.Google ScholarPubMed
Radloff, L. S. (1977) The CES–D scale. A self-report depression score for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurements, 1, 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, C. S., Robinson, D. R. & Hausmann, L. R. M. (2001) Stereotyping among providers and consumers of public mental health services – the role of perceived group variability. Behavior Modification, 25, 406442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sobel, M. (1982) Asymptomatic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equations models. In Sociological Methodology (ed. Leinhart, S.), pp. 290312. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Walker, I. & Read, J. (2002) The differential effectiveness of psychosocial and biogenetic causal explanations in reducing negative attitudes toward ‘mental illness'. Psychiatry, 65, 313325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, J. E., Robins, L. N., Bushnell, J. A., et al (1994) Perceived barriers to care in St Louis (USA) and Christchurch (NZ): reasons for not seeking professional help for psychological distress. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 29, 155164.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Item-total correlations for the personal and perceived depression stigma scales for the trial group prior to randomisation (n=525)

Figure 1

Table 2 Personal and perceived stigma scores for each intervention group over time

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.