Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T19:02:06.508Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Affective problems in adults with mild learning disability: the roles of social disadvantage and ill health

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Stephan Collishaw*
Affiliation:
Medical Research Council Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London
Barbara Maughan
Affiliation:
Medical Research Council Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London
Andrew Pickles
Affiliation:
School of Epidemiology and Health Science, and Centre for Census and Survey Research, University of Manchester, UK
*
Dr Stephan Collishaw, Box Number PO46, Institute of Psychiatry, 16 De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK. Tel: +44 (0)20 78480487; e-mail: s.collishaw@iop.kcl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Mild learning disability is associated with an increased risk of affective disorder. This study examines the extent to which adult socio-economic disadvantage and ill health contribute to this risk. Samples were drawn from the 1958 National Child Development Study. Relative to a comparison group, mild learning disability at age 11 was associated with elevated rates of depressive symptoms throughout adult life, and carried a six-fold risk of chronic depressed mood. The group difference in depressed mood at age 43 years was in large part mediated by variations in adult socio-economic disadvantage and ill health.

Type
Short Report
Copyright
Copyright © 2004 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Epidemiological data show increased risks for depressed mood (Reference Maughan, Collishaw and PicklesMaughan et al, 1999) and affective disorders (Reference Richards, Maughan and HardyRichards et al, 2001) in adults with mild learning disability. However, the mechanisms underlying the link between mild learning disability and depression are poorly understood. As noted in a government White Paper (Department of Health, 2001), people with learning disability are among the most socially excluded, and often have greater health needs than the rest of the population. One possibility, then, is that social disadvantage and physical ill health contribute to the risk for depression in adults with mild learning disability. We test this model using data from a British birth cohort followed to age 43 years.

METHOD

Samples

The National Child Development Study (NCDS; Reference Bynner, Butler and FerriBynner et al, 2000) includes all the children born in the UK during the week 3–9 March 1958. Study members have been assessed regularly from birth to age 43 years. Mild learning disability was defined at age 11 years using a standardised classroom-administered general ability test on the basis of scores equivalent to an IQ of 70 or less. As the test showed least discrimination at the lowest extreme of the range, school placement data at ages 11 years and 16 years were used to exclude children with severe learning disabilities: see Maughan et al (Reference Maughan, Collishaw and Pickles1999) for details. Of 13 473 children with relevant data, 48 (3.6 per 1000) were classified as having severe disability, 275 (20.4 per 1000) as having mild learning disability, and all others (n=13 150) were assigned to the non-learning-disabled comparison group.

Adult depressed mood and service use for depression

The 15-item version of the Malaise Inventory (Reference Ruttel, Tizard and WhitmoreRutter et al, 1970; Reference Grant, Nolan and EllisGrant et al, 1990) was used to assess depressed mood at ages 23, 33 and 43 years. This scale has good reliability and validity, performing well in identifying individuals with current affective disorder (Reference Rodgers, Pickles and PowerRodgers et al, 1999). In this study the scale showed good reliability for both mild learning disability and comparison groups at all ages (α=0.75–0.87), and related equally well to reports of service contacts for depression in the two groups. Scores of 5 or more were used to index high levels of depressed mood. Chronic depressed mood refers to a high score at all three adult contacts. The interview at age 43 assessed contacts with general practitioners or specialists for depressive problems over the previous 10 years.

Childhood social disadvantage

Indicators of family size, social class and housing tenure at age 11 years and institutional or substitute care up to 16 years were combined into an eight-point childhood social disadvantage index (see Reference Maughan, Collishaw and PicklesMaughan et al, 1999).

Adult socio-economic disadvantage

A five-point index of adult socio-economic disadvantage at age 43 years was created (a count of indicators of semi-skilled or unskilled social class; current unemployment; no academic or vocational qualification; rented accommodation; receipt of income-related benefits).

Adult self-rated health

Participants rated their current health at age 43 years on a four-point scale, with ratings of fair/poor (v. good/excellent) considered to be a valid index of ill health (Reference Manor, Matthews and PowerManor et al, 2001).

RESULTS

Response rates and attrition

Complete data on the childhood measures were available for 232 of the mild learning disability sample and 11 899 of the comparison group. Of these, 102 (44.0%) and 8385 (70.5%) responded at age 43 years. Complete data for the multivariate models were available for 86 adults with mild learning disability and 7880 comparison group members. The measure of chronic depressed mood was available for 60 and 6513 individuals in the respective groups. Individuals with mild learning disability had a higher death rate by age 43 years (5.2% v. 2.6%; OR=2.1, 95% CI 1.2–3.8; P=0.02), were less likely to be traced and more often refused to participate. Response rates were lower for men, for individuals from socially deprived childhood backgrounds, for those who had received special education and those with childhood behavioural problems. Weights were constructed to mitigate these response biases, ensuring that findings were as representative as possible of the groups originally selected in childhood.

Adult depressed mood and service use for depression

In keeping with previous findings from this cohort (Reference Maughan, Collishaw and PicklesMaughan et al, 1999), mild learning disability continued to confer a significantly increased risk for depressed mood at age 43 years for both men (43% v. 16%; OR=3.92, 95% CI 2.2–6.9) and women (42% v. 24%; OR=2.38, 95% CI 1.3–4.3). Risk for chronic depressed mood at ages 23, 33 and 43 years was particularly elevated (15% v. 3%; OR = 6.35, 95% CI 2.6–15.6). Consultations for depression with general practitioners or specialists were also more common in the group with mild learning disability (32% v. 21%; OR=1.78, 95% CI 1.2–2.7).

Influences on affect at mid-life

As reported by Maughan et al (Reference Maughan, Collishaw and Pickles1999), individuals with mild learning disability in the NCDS often came from deprived family backgrounds. This disadvantage continued into adult life – at age 43 years, mean scores on the social disadvantage index were over three times higher in the group with mild learning disability, with many experiencing multiple social difficulties (64% v. 13%; OR=11.6, 95% CI 7.4–18.3). Adults with mild learning disability also more commonly rated their health as fair or poor (41% v. 18%; OR=3.5, 95% CI 2.3–5.3). Adult social disadvantage and self-rated health were strongly associated with Malaise Inventory scores at age 43 years in both groups (P<0.0001).

Models of the extent to which group differences in depressed mood at age 43 years were attributable to these effects are summarised in Table 1. A base model (controlled for gender and childhood social background) showed a group difference in depressed affect with an odds ratio of 2.43. Models 2 and 3 added controls for socio-economic disadvantage and self-rated health at age 43 years, reducing this group difference to odds ratios of 1.57 and 1.95 respectively. Together (model 4) these predictors explained a substantial proportion of the group difference in Malaise Inventory scores, with the effect of mild learning disability status reduced to non-significance (OR=1.43; P=0.15). Overall, adult social circumstances and ill health reduced the model coefficient for the relationship between mild learning disability and depression by 60% (b=0.887 in model 1 to b=0.358 in model 4; P=0.03).

Table 1 Models of group differences in depressive affect at age 43 years

Risk of high Malaise Inventory score at 43 years (mild learning disability v. comparison group) Percentage of group difference explained (reference, model 1)
Model OR (95% CI) P (%)
1 2.43 (1.5-3.9) <0.001
2 1.57 (0.96-2.6) 0.07 49
3 1.95 (1.2-3.1) 0.005 25
4 1.43 (0.88-2.3) 0.15 60

DISCUSSION

Like previous reports from the UK birth cohorts (Reference Maughan, Collishaw and PicklesMaughan et al, 1999; Reference Richards, Maughan and HardyRichards et al, 2001), we found a strong association between mild learning disability and depressive symptoms in adult life. Reports of depression-related service use followed a similar pattern. Repeated assessments highlighted the chronicity of these problems: mild learning disability was associated with a six-fold increased risk of depressed mood at all three adult assessment points.

Two limitations of the study must be noted. First, sample attrition was high, particularly among individuals with mild learning disability. Although we used weights to mitigate the effects of measured response biases, it remains possible that unmeasured characteristics biased response measured in other ways. Second, the Malaise Inventory assesses depressed mood, so inferences to depressive disorders need to be made with caution. Queries also arise over the extent to which questionnaire measures such as the Malaise Inventory assess depressed mood in an ‘IQ-fair’ way. However, results from this inventory correspond well with the elevated rates of service use for adult depression reported in this study, and with interviewer-based diagnoses of affective disorder reported by Richards et al (Reference Richards, Maughan and Hardy2001).

Our main aim was to test possible explanations for the links between mild learning disability and depression. Results showed that social disadvantage, and to a lesser extent physical health problems, contributed strongly to the increased risk for depressed mood at age 43 years in the mild learning disability group. These findings are important for two reasons. First, they imply that there is not necessarily an intrinsic connection between mild learning disability and depression. Second, they also provide a possible target for prevention and intervention. In particular, strategies for addressing the strong associations between socio-economic deprivation, ill health and mild learning disability (Department of Health, 2001) may hold promise for reducing levels of depressive affect in this group.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by Medical Research Council grant G9538070. B.M. is supported by the Medical Research Council. We are grateful to the UK Data Archive for access to the National Child Development Study, and to Peter Shepherd and colleagues at the Centre for Longitudinal Studies for information about death rates in our samples.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None. Funding detailed in Acknowledgements.

References

Bynner, J., Butler, N., Ferri, E., et al (2000) The Design and Conduct of the 1999–2000 Surveys of the National Child Development Study and the 1970 British Cohort Study. CLS Cohort Studies Working Paper 1. http://www.cls.ioe.ac.uk/Cohort/Ncds2000/mainncds00.htm Google Scholar
Department of Health (2001) Valuing People. A New Strategy for Learning Disability for the 21t Century. Cm 5086. London: Stationery Office. (http://www.archive.official-documents.co.uk/document/cm50/5086/5086.htm).Google Scholar
Grant, G., Nolan, M. & Ellis, N. (1990) A reappraisal of he Malaise Inventory. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 25, 170178.Google Scholar
Manor, O., Matthews, S. & Power, C. (2001) Self-rated health and longstanding illness: inter-relationships with morbidity in early adulthood. International Journal of Epidemiology, 30, 600607.Google Scholar
Maughan, B., Collishaw, S. & Pickles, A. (1999) Mild mental retardation: psychosocial functioning in adulthood. Psychological Medicine, 29, 351–66.Google Scholar
Richards, M., Maughan, B., Hardy, R., et al (2001) Long-term affective disorder in people with mild learning disability. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 523527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodgers, B., Pickles, A., Power, C., et al (1999) Validity of the Malaise Inventory in general population samples. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34, 333341.Google Scholar
Ruttel, M., Tizard, J. & Whitmore, K. (eds) (1970) Education, Health and Behaviour. London: Longman, Green.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Models of group differences in depressive affect at age 43 years

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.