Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-p566r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-26T10:24:48.728Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Causal beliefs and attitudes to people with schizophrenia

Trend analysis based on data from two population surveys in Germany

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Matthias C. Angermeyer*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Leipzig, Germany
Herbert Matschinger
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Leipzig, Germany
*
Professor Dr M.C. Angermeyer, University of Leipzig, Department of Psychiatry, Johannisallee 20, D-04317 Leipzig, Germany. Tel: +49 341 9724530; fax: +49 341 9724539; e-mail: krausem@medizin.uni-leipzig.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

It is a widely shared belief that an increase in mental health literacy will result in an improvement of attitudes towards people with mental illness.

Aims

To examine how the German public's causal attributions of schizophrenia and their desire for social distance from people with schizophrenia developed over the 1990s.

Method

A trend analysis was carried out using data from two representative population surveys conducted in the Länder constituting the former Federal Republic of Germany in 1990 and 2001.

Results

Parallel to an increase in the public's tendency to endorse biological causes, an increase in the desire for social distance from people with schizophrenia was found.

Conclusions

The assumption underlying current anti-stigma programmes that there is a positive relationship between endorsing biological causes and the acceptance of people with mental illness appears to be problematic.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 2005 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

It is a widely shared belief that an increase in the public's mental health literacy (Reference JormJorm, 2000) will result in an improvement of attitudes towards people with mental illness. Most of the current anti-stigma programmes are based on this rationale (Reference Lopez-IborLopez-Ibor, 2002). In particular, promoting biological concepts as a causal explanation for mental disorders is considered to be a promising strategy. The argument is that if the causes of mental disorders were attributed to factors outside the individuals’ control, people's reactions to those with mental illness would be less negative. This assumption has not been empirically tested until now. In this study, based on a trend analysis of data from two representative population surveys in Germany, we therefore investigate whether the greater the public's endorsement of biological causes of schizophrenia, the less is its desire for social distance from people with schizophrenia.

METHOD

In the spring of 1990 a representative survey was conducted in what was then the Federal Republic of Germany among German citizens aged 18 years and older, living in non-institutional settings. The sample was drawn using a three-stage sampling procedure, with sample points (electoral wards) at the first stage, households at the second stage and individuals within the target households at the third stage. Target households within the sample point were determined according to the random route procedure: that is, a street was selected randomly as a starting point from where the interviewer followed a set route through the area (Reference Gabler and Hoffmeyer-ZlotnikGabler & Hoffmeyer-Zlotnik, 1997). Within the target households, the interviewees were selected by means of random digits. A total of 2118 interviews were conducted, reflecting a response rate of 71.9%.

In 2001 a second representative survey was conducted; this time the sampling frame differed, with the sample being drawn from the whole of Germany, the number of interviews conducted in the West and the East reflecting the proportions of the population living in the two parts of the country. Apart from this, the same three-stage sampling procedure was used as in the previous survey. In total, 5025 interviews were conducted (reflecting a response rate of 65.1%), 4020 of which were obtained in the States (Länder) representing the former Federal Republic of Germany. The socio-demographic characteristics of both samples are shown in Table 1. For comparison, data from the Official Registry for the total population are also provided; no major difference is apparent.

Table 1 Socio-demographic characteristics of the population samples

1990 2001
Survey (%) Total population1 (%) Survey (%) Total population2 (%)
Gender
     Male 45.5 48.5 43.8 48.3
     Female 54.5 51.5 56.2 51.7
Age, years
     18-25 11.4 12.3 11.8 9.8
     26-45 36.4 38.0 38.7 37.8
     46-60 25.0 24.2 24.1 23.8
     61+ 27.2 25.5 25.4 29.1
Educational attainment
     Still a student 0.63 0.44 0.33 0.24
     No school completed 3.73 2.54 3.83 2.14
     Hauptschule (9 years of school completed) 54.73 55.84 45.23 49.14
     Realschule/Polytechnische Oberschule 24.83 25.84 32.53 27.54
     (10 years of school completed)
     Fachhochschulreife/Abitur (technical college of higher education/A-levels) 16.83 15.54 18.13 21.14
Marital status
     Married 60.9 61.2 55.2 56.5
     Divorced 3.1 5.4 8.9 7.5
     Widowed 14.6 10.6 11.0 9.2
     Single 21.4 22.8 24.8 26.8

Interview

In both surveys a largely identical personal, fully structured interview was conducted. Therefore, no pre-testing was necessary for the second survey. In both surveys, the same vignettes and the same instruments were applied and used for analyses.

Vignettes

At the beginning of the interview respondents were presented with a vignette of a diagnostically unlabelled psychiatric case history, depicting a case of either schizophrenia or major depressive disorder. The symptoms described in the vignettes fulfilled the criteria of DSM–III–R (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) for the respective disorder. Before the vignettes were used in the survey, each was independently rated by five experts on psychopathology (psychiatrists or psychologists) masked to the actual diagnosis, providing confirmation of the correct diagnosis for each case history. The respondents were randomly allocated to receive one of the two vignettes. In this analysis only data from interviews containing the vignette depicting schizophrenia have been used (1990 survey, n=511; 2001 survey, n=1987).

Causal attributions

Respondents’ attributions of the cause of the schizophrenic disorder depicted in the vignette were assessed by responses to eight items, with two items each referencing psychosocial stress (life event, stress at work), biological causes (brain disease, heredity), conditions of socialisation (broken home, lack of parental affection) or causes that individuals could influence themselves (lack of willpower, immoral lifestyle). Using a five-point Likert scale ranging from ‘definitely a cause’ to ‘definitely no cause’, respondents were asked to indicate how relevant they considered each potential cause to be.

Social distance

For the assessment of respondents’ desire for social distance we used a scale developed by Link et al (Reference Link, Cullen and Frank1987), a modified version of the Bogardus Social Distance Scale (Reference BogardusBogardus, 1925). It includes seven items representing the following social relationships: tenant, co-worker, neighbour, member of the same social circle, person one would recommend for a job, in-law and child carer. Using a five-point Likert scale ranging from ‘in any case’ to ‘in no case at all’, the respondents could indicate to what extent they would, in the situation presented, accept the person described in the vignette.

Statistical analysis

To test the effect of time on the two combined response categories, indicating either agreement or disagreement, logit models were estimated with each of the items, assessing people's causal attributions and their desire for social distance. The year 2001 served as the reference category. All effects were controlled for the effect of gender, age and educational attainment. In order to take into account the multistage sampling procedure, the analyses were carried out with SVYLOGIT of STATA version 8 SE (Stata, 2003), using the 510 sample points as primary sampling units. Owing to the large sample sizes, the power of statistical tests was quite high; effect sizes therefore were also calculated.

RESULTS

Causal beliefs and preference for social distance in 1990

In 1990 the German public's beliefs about the causes of schizophrenia were quite discrepant from what mental health professionals consider to be of aetiological relevance (Table 2). Psychosocial stress was particularly popular among the public: acute stress (life events) ranked first and chronic stress (stress at work) second. In contrast, biological factors ranked fifth (brain disease) and sixth (hereditary factors). Hereditary factors, for which the strongest scientific evidence exists, were endorsed as an explanation for the occurrence of the disorder less frequently than lack of willpower or broken home; only lack of parental affection and immoral lifestyle were chosen less frequently.

Table 2 Causal attributions of the population samples in 1990 and 2001

1990 (n=511) (%)2 2001 (n=1987) (%)2 Odds ratio1 (95% CI) P Effect size (absolute value)
Brain disease 51.0 70.0 0.443 (0.333-0.592) <0.001 0.387
Heredity 40.9 60.2 0.447 (0.335-0.597) <0.001 0.387
Life event 73.8 72.3 1.070 (0.777-1.461) 0.692 0.032
Stress at work 62.0 57.7 1.182 (0.891-1.568) 0.246 0.086
Broken home 55.5 39.3 1.889 (1.437-2.482) <0.001 0.323
Lack of parental affection 36.0 30.4 1.290 (0.943-1.764) 0.111 0.119
Lack of willpower 51.6 36.4 1.838 (1.394-2.424) <0.001 0.308
Immoral lifestyle 18.2 20.9 0.803 (0.559-1.151) 0.232 0.070

Change in beliefs during the 1990s

During the 1990s the most pronounced changes occurred with regard to biological causes. The percentage of those who endorsed brain disease and hereditary factors as a cause increased substantially, from 55% to 70% and from 45% to 60%, respectively. With values of 0.39, effect sizes indicated a small to medium effect. The attribution to psychosocial stress remained practically unchanged. However, the readiness to blame the individual for the illness decreased considerably. In 2001 the respondents attributed the cause less frequently to a lack of will power than 11 years before, and the same applies to the conditions under which the individual grew up, with a broken home being less frequently assumed to be a cause. In conclusion, one can state that during the 1990s the gap between mental health professionals’ views on the origin of schizophrenia and public beliefs became narrower. There is a stronger tendency towards adopting the causal explanations approved of by psychiatry, whereas those that are disapproved of are more frequently rejected (Table 2).

Changes in desire for social distance during the 1990s

In sharp contrast to the changes observed with regard to the public's causal attributions was the development of the public's desire for social distance from people with schizophrenia. Rather than having decreased, as one might expect, the desire for social distance in all social relationships studied increased markedly (Table 3). For example, whereas 19% of the respondents rejected an individual with schizophrenia as a neighbour in 1990, this number had risen to 35% in 2001; in 1990 about 44% would not rent a room to someone with schizophrenia but in 2001 this proportion amounted to 63%.

Table 3 Social distance desired by the population samples in 1990 and 2001

1990 (n=511) (%)2 2001 (n=1987) (%)2 Odds ratio1 (95% CI) P Effect size (absolute value)
Tenant 43.8 62.9 0.456 (0.340-0.612) <0.001 0.367
Co-worker 20.2 33.4 0.493 (0.345-0.706) <0.001 0.303
Neighbour 19.3 35.2 0.429 (0.298-0.617) <0.001 0.358
Child carer 68.3 83.9 0.414 (0.299-0.573) <0.001 0.371
Marriage into one's family 58.9 65.7 0.722 (0.541-0.964) 0.027 0.143
Member of the same social circle 45.1 55.6 0.628 (0.469-0.841) 0.002 0.215
Person one would recommend for a job 46.0 60.4 0.550 (0.410-0.739) <0.001 0.294

DISCUSSION

The question implied in our introduction – whether the greater the German public's endorsement of biological causes of schizophrenia the less is its desire for social distance from people with schizophrenia – has to be answered in the negative. No such relationship could be observed. On the contrary, both biological attributions and social distance were positively related with each other. Although the endorsement of biological causes increased substantially, the public's rejection of people with schizophrenia increased in the same period. To our knowledge, there exists no other study with which a direct comparison could be made. The nearest is one from the USA showing that, despite the fact that over a period of 50 years people had developed a somewhat more differentiated view of mental disorders, the stereotype of the dangerousness of the person with mental illness had increased substantially (Reference Phelan, Link and StuevePhelan et al, 2000).

Time trends in major depression

The question arises as to whether our findings are specific for schizophrenia or apply to other mental disorders as well. Comparison with data for major depression derived from the same survey reveals that the changes concerning the endorsement of biological factors were here far less pronounced: in 2001, brain disease was assumed to be a cause as frequently as in 1990, and the tendency to attribute the cause to hereditary factors increased only slightly (effect size 0.15 as compared with 0.39 for schizophrenia). The desire for social distance remained virtually unchanged across all social relationships over the study period. Thus, the time trends of causal attributions and the desire for social distance with regard to major depression differed markedly from those found with schizophrenia. However, the observation that there was hardly any change of causal attributions and social distance is not inconsistent with the notion of a positive association between the two that has been observed with schizophrenia.

Relationship between causal attributions and desire for social distance

The results of our trend analysis correspond with those of cross-sectional analyses of the data from 1990 and 2001. In both surveys, an assumption that biological factors were a cause of schizophrenia was positively associated with a greater preference for social distance. Moreover, in 2001 this association was significantly stronger than in 1990. A more detailed analysis revealed that the more respondents endorsed biological factors (particularly brain disease) as a cause, the more lacking in self-control, unpredictable and dangerous they believed individuals with schizophrenia to be. This, in turn, was associated with a higher degree of fear, which resulted in a stronger desire for social distance (Reference Dietrich, Matschinger and AngermeyerDietrich et al, 2005).

Societal changes and their influences on social distance

Apart from public beliefs, other factors might have influenced people's attitudes towards those with mental illness. During the 1990s, mainly due to reunification, major societal changes took place in Germany; for example, the unemployment rate increased substantially. However, this should not have affected the public's attitudes to people with schizophrenia, since in our samples no significant association between the respondents’ employment status and their desire for social distance could be found. Cutbacks in health services and other aspects of the welfare state, which might also have an effect on public attitudes to people with mental illness, became effective only after our second survey had been completed. In the early 1990s a number of violent acts against prominent figures had been committed in Germany by individuals who were mentally ill. Although following these incidents negative attitudes of the public to people with schizophrenia increased, they did not persist at the same level during the subsequent years. Thus, it seems unlikely that this could be responsible for the stronger desire for social distance expressed by the respondents in 2001 (Reference Angermeyer and MatschingerAngermeyer & Matschinger, 1996; Reference Angermeyer and SchulzeAngermeyer & Schulze, 2001).

Implications for anti-stigma interventions

In conclusion, one has to say that we are facing a dilemma. On the one hand, there are good reasons for improving the public's mental health literacy by informing them about the views shared by mental health professionals on the aetiology of schizophrenia, as they may have a positive effect on people's readiness to seek professional help (Reference Angermeyer, Matschinger and Riedel-HellerAngermeyer et al, 1999); on the other, promulgating biological factors as a cause of the disorder may lead to more instead of less rejection. Against the backdrop of our findings, the idea underlying some current anti-stigma programmes – namely that by educating people about biological causes of schizophrenia their attitudes will automatically improve – appears rather problematic. Our concern is that, in the end, the opposite of what was originally intended may occur.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. People with schizophrenia now face more rejection than a decade ago.

  2. Change towards more biological causal attributions of schizophrenia is not associated with more positive attitudes towards the patients.

  3. Educating people about the biological causes of schizophrenia may not improve their attitudes towards people with schizophrenia.

LIMITATIONS

  1. This is not a panel study, which would have allowed us to examine the development over time of causal attributions and the desire for social distance on an individual level.

  2. Only the relationship between causal attributions and social distance was examined; other aspects of mental health literacy (e.g. perceived risk, treatability) and other forms of discrimination (e.g. structural discrimination) remained unconsidered.

  3. Before our findings are generalised, they need to be replicated in other countries.

Acknowledgement

The study was funded by the German Research Association (grant AN. 101/5–1).

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None. Funding detailed in Acknowledgement.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn, revised) (DSM–III–R). Washington DC: APA.Google Scholar
Angermeyer, M. C. & Matschinger, H. (1996) The effect of violent attacks by schizophrenic persons on the attitude of the public towards the mentally ill. Social Science and Medicine, 43, 17211728.Google Scholar
Angermeyer, M. C. & Schulze, B. (2001) Reinforcing stereotypes: the focus on forensic cases in news reporting and its influence on public attitudes towards the mentally ill. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 24, 469486.Google Scholar
Angermeyer, M.C., Matschinger, H. & Riedel-Heller, S. G. (1999) Whom to ask for help in case of mental disorder? Preferences of the lay public. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34, 202210.Google Scholar
Bogardus, E. S. (1925) Measuring social distances. Journal of Applied Sociology, 9, 299308.Google Scholar
Dietrich, S., Matschinger, H. & Angermeyer, M.C. (2005) The relationship between biogenetic causal explanations and social distance toward people with mental disorders. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, in press.Google Scholar
Gabler, S. & Hoffmeyer-Zlotnik, J. H. P. (1997) Stichproben in der Umfragenpraxis. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag.Google Scholar
Jorm, A. F. (2000) Mental health literacy: public knowledge and beliefs about mental disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 396401.Google Scholar
Link, B. G., Cullen, F. T., Frank, J., et al (1987) The social rejection of former mental patients: understanding why labels matter. American Journal of Sociology, 92, 14611500.Google Scholar
Lopez-Ibor, J. J. (2002) The WPA and the fight against stigma because of mental illness. World Psychiatry, 1, 3031.Google Scholar
Phelan, J.C., Link, B. G., Stueve, A., et al (2000) Public conception of mental illness in 1950 and 1996: what is mental illness and is it to be feared? Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 41, 188207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stata (2003) Stata Statistical Software: Release 8.0. College Station, TX: Stata Corporation.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Socio-demographic characteristics of the population samples

Figure 1

Table 2 Causal attributions of the population samples in 1990 and 2001

Figure 2

Table 3 Social distance desired by the population samples in 1990 and 2001

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.