Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-m8qmq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-20T10:22:37.923Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive performance in presumed obligate carriers for psychosis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Timothea Toulopoulou*
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine
Francesca Mapua-Filbey
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine
Seema Quraishi
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine
Eugenia Kravariti
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine
Robin G. Morris
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology
Colm McDonald
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry London, UK
Muriel Walshe
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry London, UK
Elvira Bramon
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry London, UK
Robin M. Murray
Affiliation:
Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry London, UK
*
Dr Timothea Toulopoulou, Section of General Psychiatry Box 63, Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK. Tel: +44(0) 207 848 0061; fax: +44(0)207 701 9044; e-mail: t.toulopoulou@iop.kcl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

We report cognitive performance of a group of individuals who are likely to have transmitted liability to psychosis to their offspring. Out of 230 relatives of patients with psychosis, 27 met our criteria for a presumed obligate carrier, that is a non-psychotic individual who had a parent or a sibling as well as an offspring with psychosis. The presumed obligate carriers showed impairments in verbal memory and in visuospatial manipulations, suggesting that these individuals transmit vulnerability for psychosis to their offspring in terms of a disability to recall verbal information and an impaired capacity to perceive spatial relations.

Type
Short Report
Copyright
Copyright © 2005 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Deficits in cognition may represent a genetic vulnerability for schizophrenia because cognitive deficits, particularly in verbal learning and memory, are found in both individuals with schizophrenia and their healthy relatives (Reference Byrne, Clafferty and CoswayByrne et al, 2003; Toulopoulou et al, Reference Toulopoulou, Morris and Rabe-Hesketh2003a , Reference Toulopoulou, Rabe-Hesketh and King b ). A similar pattern has emerged in bipolar disorder, with both patients and their healthy relatives showing decreased performance in certain aspects of cognition (Reference Keri, Kelemen and BenedekKeri et al, 2001). The distinction of psychosis into schizophrenia and bipolar disorder has been increasingly questioned, with recent evidence suggesting that the two disorders share some common characteristics in terms of cognition and genetic liability (Reference Murray, Sham and van OsMurray et al, 2004). We explore further the idea that specific cognitive dysfunction may be a marker of genetic susceptibility to psychosis by assessing a very selected group of relatives derived from a large sample of pedigrees containing multiple cases of psychosis. The relatives had passed the normal age threshold for developing psychosis (mean age=57.2 years, s.d.=6.5) and, although healthy themselves, the structure of their pedigrees suggests that they are likely carriers of susceptibility genes for psychosis.

METHOD

Participants and procedures

Our sample comprised a subgroup of first-degree relatives (n=27) of individuals with schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder with psychotic features. The subgroup had been identified as being more likely to have transmitted the liability to schizophrenia and/or bipolar disorder (obligate carriers for psychosis) because, although healthy themselves, they have a parent or sibling, as well as an offspring, with schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder. The sample constitutes a sub-population of a larger cohort of relatives of individuals with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and controls from the Maudsley Family Study of Psychosis (Toulopoulou et al, Reference Toulopoulou, Morris and Rabe-Hesketh2003a , Reference Toulopoulou, Rabe-Hesketh and King b , Reference Toulopolou, Quraishi and McDonald2005; Reference McDonald, Bullmore and ShamMcDonald et al, 2004). The 27 presumed obligate carriers were from a total of 230 non-psychotic relatives who were neurocognitively assessed, and derived from 27 separate families. Eleven of these non-psychotic relatives were from families with documented multiple cases of schizophrenia, six from families with documented multiple cases of bipolar disorder (with at least one member with bipolar I disorder with psychotic features), and ten from families with documented cases of both bipolar I disorder with psychotic features and schizophrenia. After complete description of the study to the participants, written informed consent was obtained. The study was approved by the South London and Maudsley NHS Trust/Institute of Psychiatry Ethical Committee (Research). A control group comprised 32 individuals with no family history of schizophrenia or bipolar disorder, matched for age, gender, education and handedness.

Structured diagnostic interviews were administered to all participants using the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia–Lifetime version (Reference Spitzer, Endicott and RobinsSpitzer et al, 1978). Information regarding the timing and nature of any psychopathology was collected to enable DSM–IV diagnoses to be made (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Eight of the presumed obligate carriers had fulfilled criteria for a DSM–IV Axis I disorder at some point in their lives, five for a major depressive disorder, two for panic disorder and one for anxiety disorder; one also fulfilled criteria for schizotypal personality disorder. Three of the controls had also fulfilled criteria for a major depressive disorder at some point in their lives. All participants were recovered and were not in treatment at the time of assessment.

The neuropsychological tests assessed:

  1. (a) IQ, as measured by a five sub-test short form (vocabulary, similarities, comprehension, block design, object assembly) of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (WAIS–R); Reference WechslerWechsler, 1981; and

  2. (b) verbal and visual memory and learning, as measured by (i) immediate recall, (ii) 30-min delayed recall and (iii) saving scores of two stories and designs – two procedures taken from the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS; Reference Wechsler and StoneWechsler & Stone, 1945).

The savings score was calculated using the following formula: saving score= (delayed recall/immediate recall)×100.

Data analyses

All data were analysed with independent-samples two-tailed t-tests except for handedness and gender where Pearson's χ2 tests were used. Analyses were performed primarily to compare the presumed obligate carriers with the controls. We also re-ran all the analyses after excluding all those with a history of psychiatric disorder. We report findings without correcting for multiple testing as we consider Bonferroni corrections too conservative since many of the tests are correlated and not independent. Furthermore, although the presumed obligate sample is abstracted from a very large population of relatives, the numbers are sufficiently low to warrant initially less-stringent criteria.

RESULTS

The groups were matched and therefore compatible in terms of age (t=–0.66, d.f.=58, P=0.51), gender (χ2=0.06, d.f.=1, P=0.81), education (t=–0.88, d.f.=57, P=0.38) and handedness (χ2=2.5, d.f.=1, P=0.12). We found no differences between the presumed obligate carriers for psychosis and the controls in estimated IQ and visual memory (Table 1). However, the presumed obligate carriers scored lower than the controls on object assembly, a task that assesses perception of visual relationships, and on the delayed recall of verbal memory. A trend was also found for immediate recall of verbal memory to be poorer in obligate carriers. This became significant after excluding all those with a history of psychiatric disorder (t=2.86, d.f.=42, P=0.007). The differences in delayed recall of verbal memory remained significant even after excluding all those with a history of psychiatric disorder (t=2.5, d.f.=41, P=0.02), but the significant difference in the object assembly disappeared after excluding those with a history of psychiatric disorder (t=1.8, d.f.=44, P=0.08).

Table 1 Current general intellectual and verbal and visual memory function (means (s.d.)) for presumed obligate carriers and normal controls

Presumed obligate carriers (n=27) Controls (n=32)
WAIS–R IQ 106.3 (14.2) 109.6 (11.5
Verbal sub-tests
    Vocabulary 10.9 (2.6) 11.2 (2.5)
    Similarities 9.5 (2.5) 10.1 (1.7)
    Comprehension 10.8 (3.1) 11.2 (2.7
Performance sub-tests
    Block design 9.6 (2.9) 10.09 (2.08)
    Object assembly 7.3 (2.4)* 8.8 (2.7)
Verbal memory
    Immediate recall1 9.96 (3.9) 11.6 (3.3)
    Delayed recall2 7.3 (3.5) 9.2 (3.6)
    Saving scores2 72.9 (20.10) 77.8 (15.8)
Visual memory
    Immediate recall1 8.2 (3.9) 8.7 (3.2)
    Delayed recall1 6.3 (2.9) 6.5 (3.3)
    Saving scores1 82.6 (22.5) 77.2 (29.9)

DISCUSSION

The presumed obligate carriers for psychosis represent a group who, although not suffering from psychosis themselves, are likely to have transmitted to their offspring a liability to psychosis. This group had a similar IQ and educational attainment to controls, with both groups falling within the average range of ability. However, they performed worse on object assembly, a test that evaluates visual – motor speed and visuospatial manipulation. Furthermore, the presumed obligate carriers showed decreased performance in delayed recall of the logical memory sub-test, a test that assesses long-term verbal memory. These findings remained statistically significant when we excluded those with any Axis I disorder; moreover, when we excluded those with an Axis I disorder differences between the immediate recall of the logical memory sub-test also became statistically significant. Consistent with some studies of those with bipolar disorder and their relatives (Reference Quraishi and FrangouQuraishi & Frangou, 2002) and of the relatives of those with schizophrenia (Reference Laurent, d'Amato and NaegeleLaurent et al, 2000), visual memory was not impaired among our presumed obligate carrier subjects, suggesting that this does not reflect genetic liability to psychosis.

To conclude, our data suggest that presumed obligate carriers for psychosis transmit liability to their offspring in the form of impairments in verbal memory and ability to perceive spatial relationships. These impairments cannot be explained by an overall decreased level of general intellectual ability or by deprivations related to education.

Acknowledgements

E.B. and C.McD. were supported by research training fellowships from the Wellcome Trust.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None. Funding detailed in Acknowledgements.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Byrne, M., Clafferty, B. A., Cosway, R., et al (2003) Neuropsychology, genetic liability, and psychotic symptoms in those at high risk of schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 3848.Google Scholar
Keri, S., Kelemen, O., Benedek, G., et al (2001) Different trait markers for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, a neurocognitive approach. Psychological Medicine, 31, 915922.Google Scholar
Laurent, A., d'Amato, T., Naegele, B., et al (2000) Executive functioning and memory in first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Encephale-Revue de Psychiatrie Clinique Biologique et Therapeutique, 26, 6774.Google Scholar
McDonald, C., Bullmore, E. T., Sham, P. C., et al (2004) Association of genetic risks for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder with specific and generic brain structural endophenotypes. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 974984.Google Scholar
Murray, R. M., Sham, P., van Os, J., et al (2004) A developmental model for similarities and dissimilarities between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia Research, 71, 405416.Google Scholar
Quraishi, S. & Frangou, S. (2002) Neuropsychology of bipolar disorder: a review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 72, 209226.Google Scholar
Spitzer, R. L., Endicott, J. & Robins, E. (1978) Research diagnostic criteria, rationale and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 773782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toulopoulou, T., Morris, R. G., Rabe-Hesketh, S., et al (2003a) Selectivity of verbal memory deficit in schizophrenic patients and their relatives. American Journal of Medical Genetics B Neuropsychiatry Genetics, 116B, 17.Google Scholar
Toulopoulou, T., Rabe-Hesketh, S., King, H., et al (2003b) Episodic memory in schizophrenic patients and their relatives Schizophrenia Research, 63, 261271.Google Scholar
Toulopolou, T., Quraishi, S., McDonald, C., et al (2005) The Maudsley Family Study: Premorbid and current general intellectual function levels in familial bipolar I disorder and schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology in press.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1981) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised Manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. & Stone, C. P. (1945) Manual for the Wechsler Memory Scale. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Current general intellectual and verbal and visual memory function (means (s.d.)) for presumed obligate carriers and normal controls

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.