Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-cfpbc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T02:05:00.651Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Authors' reply

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

M. Wagner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Bonn, Sigmund-Freud Strasse 25, 53105 Bonn, Germany E-mail: michael.wagner@ukb.uni-bonn.de
S. Schulze-Rauschenbach
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Bonn, Sigmund-Freud Strasse 25, 53105 Bonn, Germany E-mail: michael.wagner@ukb.uni-bonn.de
T. Schlaepfer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Bonn, Sigmund-Freud Strasse 25, 53105 Bonn, Germany E-mail: michael.wagner@ukb.uni-bonn.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © 2005 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

We are glad that Fregni et al share our interest in side-effect profiles of rTMS and ECT in major depression and thank them for their positive judgement of our work. They draw attention to their related study (O'Connor et al, 2003), which we regretfully overlooked when we wrote our article. They point to similarities between the studies, but speculate about the reasons for the discrepant findings regarding the clinical efficacy of rTMS. We believe the following methodological differences might contribute.

First, in the study by O'Connor et al (2003), the level of baseline depression was different in the treatment groups: those receiving rTMS were significantly less depressed than those receiving ECT. Furthermore, those treated with rTMS were medication-free for at least 2 weeks but those receiving ECT continued to receive antidepressant medication. Finally, the duration of treatment – and the interval between initial and follow-up measurements – tended to be longer (2–4 weeks) in the ECT group than in the rTMS group (2 weeks). These features most likely contributed to the better clinical efficacy of unilateral ECT compared with rTMS in the study by O'Connor et al, where not a single patient treated with rTMS showed a clinically significant (50% reduction in the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression) response.

In contrast, those treated with either rTMS or ECT in our study were matched for baseline levels of depression. They were treated for about 5 weeks on average. Antidepressant medication was kept constant in both ECT and rTMS treatment arms, and both treatments were clinically effective in about half of the patients. In principle, a comparative study of side-effects of two treatments only seems to be relevant when both modalities have a measurable clinical effect.

We agree that the effects of rTMS on mood and cognition may be independent of each other, and may point to different neural networks mediating these effects. However, the better retrograde memory performance after treatment, even in patients lacking an antidepressant response to rTMS, reported by O'Connor et al, is not necessarily suggestive of such a dissociation. It might also be explained by test repetition effects, which were masked in the ECT group because of enduring memory impairments. A healthy control group assessed repeatedly can be used to control for this confounding variable. We noted that patients receiving rTMS did not show stronger improvements over time than the control group for any objective cognitive measure, effectively ruling out a genuine memory-enhancing effect of rTMS as used in our study.

With the development of magnetic seizure therapy as possibly yet another form of brain stimulation for depression, the issue of relative benefits, side-effects and the duration of both will need further careful assessment. We have highlighted some of the methodological issues to be considered when studying the effects of different treatments on cognition.

References

EDITED BY KIRIAKOS XENITIDIS

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.