Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-8mjnm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T15:21:24.773Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

La belle indifférence in conversion symptoms and hysteria

Systematic review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Jon Stone*
Affiliation:
Division of Clinical Neurosciences, School of Molecular and Clinical Medicine, University of Edinburgh, Western General Hospital, Edinburgh
Roger Smyth
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, School of Molecular and Clinical Medicine, University of Edinburgh, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh
Alan Carson
Affiliation:
Division of Clinical Neurosciences, School of Molecular and Clinical Medicine, University of Edinburgh, Western General Hospital, Edinburgh
Charles Warlow
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, School of Molecular and Clinical Medicine, University of Edinburgh, Royal Edinburgh Hospital, Edinburgh, UK
*
Dr Jon Stone, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Western General Hospital, Edinburgh EH4 2XU, UK. Tel: +44 (0) 131 537 2911; fax: +44 (0) 131 537 1132; e-mail: jstone@skull.dcn.ed.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

La belle indifférence refers to an apparent lack of concern shown by some patients towards their symptoms. It is often regarded as typical of conversion symptoms/hysteria.

Aims

To determine the frequency of la belle indifférence in studies of patients with conversion symptoms/hysteria and to determine whether it discriminates between conversion symptoms and symptoms attributable to organic disease.

Method

A systematic review of all studies published since 1965 that have reported rates of la belle indifférence in patients with conversion symptoms and/or patients with organic disease.

Results

Atotal of 11 studies were eligible for inclusion. The median frequency of la belle indifférence was 21 % (range 0–54%) in 356 patients with conversion symptoms, and 29% (range 0–60%) in 157 patients with organic disease.

Conclusions

The available evidence does not support the use of la belle indifférence to discriminate between conversion symptoms and symptoms of organic disease. The quality of the published studies is poor, with a lack of operational definitions and masked ratings. La belle indifférence should be abandoned as a clinical sign until both its definition and its utility have been clarified.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © 2006 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

La belle indifférence is defined in the DSM–IV description of conversion disorder (previously referred to as hysteria) as ‘a relative lack of concern about the nature or implications of the symptoms’ (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Although not one of the diagnostic criteria for this condition, it is the first feature mentioned in the list of ‘associated descriptive features’ and it also appears in the description of ICD–10 dissociative disorder (motor type; World Health Organization, 1992). However, the usefulness of this clinical sign remains controversial. We therefore conducted a systematic review to establish the reported frequency of la belle indifférence in patients with neurological symptoms that could not be explained by organic disease (conversion symptoms/hysteria) compared with patients with confirmed organic disease. We assessed the quality of the published studies and explored how the concept of la belle indifférence might be refined.

METHOD

Search strategy

The following databases were searched: Medline (1966 to December 2003), Cinahl (1982 to December 2003), EMBASE (1980 to December 2003) and PsycINFO (1965 to December 2003). We used all the vocabulary headings within each database for symptoms unexplained by organic disease, as well as the following text words: PSYCHOSOMATIC, PSYCHOGENIC, SOMATISATION, UNEXPLAINED, CONVERSION, NONORGANIC and DISSOC*. They were combined with text words for PARALYSIS, PARESIS, SENSORY DISTURBANCE, DEAFNESS, HEARING, VIS*, BLIND* and MOVEMENT DISORDERS. References to pseudoseizures were searched using the following text words: PSEUDOSEIZURE, NON-EPILEPTIC and HYSTERICAL ATTACK. In addition, all references under the heading ‘conversion disorder’ or with the text words HYSTERI*, INDIFFERENCE or ANOSODIAPHORIA (a term used to describe patients with cortical neglect who are indifferent to their disability) were examined. The titles and abstracts were reviewed online, and reprints of all studies that might contain data on la belle indifférence were obtained. The reference lists of all articles obtained were hand-searched for further articles published after 1965 (to match the electronic search strategy). Reports written in English, French and German were included.

Inclusion criteria

Studies were included only if they met the following criteria.

  1. (a) The patients were reported to have neurological symptoms. If these were described as unexplained, functional, non-organic or psychogenic, or were labelled as hysterical or conversion disorder, the data were placed in the ‘conversion symptoms/hysteria’ group. We included the symptoms of paralysis, weakness, sensory disturbance, movement disorder, visual loss, hearing loss and non-epileptic seizures. We also included studies reporting functional motor or sensory symptoms associated with pain but excluded studies in which unexplained pain was the sole symptom.

  2. Data could be extracted about the frequency of la belle indifférence in the sample.

  3. There were more than 10 participants in the study.

  4. The participants were over 16 years of age.

Data extraction and analysis

All reports were reviewed independently by three investigators (J.S., R.S. and A.C.). Discrepancies were resolved by a fourth and fifth adjudicator (M.S. and C.W.). Data were collected on the frequency of la belle indifférence, the setting of the study, the sampling method, the symptoms and case definition of the patients and the year of study. We calculated odds ratios for those studies that included control groups using Review Manager 4.2.7 for Windows (http://www.cc-ims.net/RevMan).

RESULTS

In total, 11 studies met the inclusion criteria (Reference Lewis and BermanLewis & Berman, 1965; Reference Raskin, Talbott and MeyersonRaskin et al, 1966; Reference Weinstein and LyerlyWeinstein & Lyerly, 1966; Reference Weinstein, Eck and LyerlyWeinstein et al, 1969; Reference BarnertBarnert, 1971; Reference DickesDickes, 1974; Reference Gould, Miller and GoldbergGould et al, 1986; Reference Kapfhammer, Buchheim and BoveKapfhammer et al, 1992; Reference Chabrol, Peresson and ClanetChabrol et al, 1995; Reference Ebel and LohmannEbel & Lohmann, 1995; Reference Sharma and ChaturvediSharma & Chaturvedi, 1995). Together these studies reported on a total of 356 patients with conversion symptoms and 157 patients with organic disease (see Table 1 and Fig. 1). Six studies were of conversion symptoms/hysteria only and four used a case–control design (Reference Raskin, Talbott and MeyersonRaskin et al, 1966; Reference Weinstein and LyerlyWeinstein & Lyerly, 1966; Reference BarnertBarnert, 1971; Reference Chabrol, Peresson and ClanetChabrol et al, 1995). One study included only patients with organic disease (Reference Gould, Miller and GoldbergGould et al, 1986). Two studies were excluded. One of these included only children (Reference Siegel and BarthelSiegel & Barthel, 1986); the other (Reference ReedReed, 1975) was excluded because it was not clear how many patients had la belle indifférence.

Fig. 1 Frequency ofla belle indifférence. Each point represents an individual study in the review, and the size of the point is related to the number of the patients in the study. The lines represent 95% binomial exact confidence intervals. ▒ Conversion symptoms/hysteria (n=356); ░ organic disease (n=157).

Table 1 Studies reporting la belle indifférence in patients with conversion symptoms and organic disease

Authors of study Year Number of patients Setting Methods Diagnosis Symptoms Definition and reliability measure of BI Overall opinion of BI
Conversion symptoms Organic disease
n BI, %1 n BI, %1
Lewis & Berman Reference Lewis and Berman1965 57 7 IP, Med NC, Re Hysteria Unknown3 No, No Negative
Raskin et al 1966 32 41 7 43 IP, OP, P C, Pr Conversion reaction W (18), MD (5), S (6), Sp (3). Disease: stroke, brain tumour, dystonia, multiple sclerosis, phenothiazine reaction, myelitis No, No Negative
Weinstein & Lyerly Reference Weinstein and Lyerly1966 14 7 17 0 IP, OP, Mil NC, Re Conversion hysteria S (6), W (3), Sp (2), D (1), MD (2)2 Disease: closed head injury No, No Uncertain
Weinstein et al Reference Weinstein, Eck and Lyerly1969 16 0 IP, N, Mil U, Pr Conversion hysteria ‘Neurological symptoms’ No, No Uncertain
Barnert Reference Barnert1971 46 54 63 14 OP, P NC, Re Conversion reaction W (22%), pain (26%), V/D (6%), NES (11%), Sp (9%), MD (11%). Disease: peptic ulcer, enteritis, ulcerative colitis, asthma, cardiac, hypertension, migraine, thyroid, rheumatoid arthritis No, No Uncertain
Dickes Reference Dickes1974 16 31 IP, P U, Re Conversion reaction W/MD/G (8), D (2), NES (1), V (2), S (1), Sp (1), pain (1) No, No Uncertain
Gould et al Reference Gould, Miller and Goldberg1986 30 27 IP, N C, Pr Neurological Disease: acute neurological problem (stroke 83%) Yes, No Negative
Kapfhammer et al Reference Kapfhammer, Buchheim and Bove1992 103 16 IP, OP, P C, Re Conversion disorder W (51%), S (43%), pain (11%) No, No Uncertain
Chabrol et al Reference Chabrol, Peresson and Clanet1995 15 40 40 60 IP, N C, Pr Conversion disorder W (7), S (2), NES (3), Mixed (3). Disease: stroke (10), brain tumour (14), epilepsy, encephalitis, vertigo, multiple sclerosis, cerebellar degeneration, CJD, radiation myelopathy, cervical myelopathy, myasthenia No, No Negative
Ebel & Lohmann Reference Ebel and Lohmann1995 22 27 IP, OP, P C, Pr Conversion disorder W/MD (15), S (1), NES (2), other (2), Sp (3) Yes, No ‘Lesser diagnostic value’
Sharma & Chaturvedi Reference Sharma and Chaturvedi1995 35 6 IP, OP, P C, Re Hysteria U (hysterical conversion 100%) No, No Uncertain

Analysis

The results of the systematic review are shown in Table 1 and Fig. 1. The median frequency of la belle indifférence in studies of 356 patients with conversion symptoms was 21% (range 0–54%). In studies of 157 patients with organic disease, the median frequency was 29% (range 0–60%). Four studies included control groups with disease. Analysis of odds ratios indicated that one controlled study found la belle indifférence to be significantly more common in hysteria (Reference BarnertBarnert, 1971), whereas the other three found no significant differences between patients with conversion symptoms and controls with organic disease (Reference Raskin, Talbott and MeyersonRaskin et al, 1966; Reference Weinstein and LyerlyWeinstein & Lyerly, 1966; Reference Chabrol, Peresson and ClanetChabrol et al, 1995). An additional study of 30 patients with only organic disease (mainly stroke) reported la belle indifférence in 27% (Reference Gould, Miller and GoldbergGould et al, 1986).

When studies were ordered by year of publication, no trend towards an increase or decrease in reporting of la belle indifférence over time was apparent. Of the 11 studies, 7 concluded that la belle indifférence was not helpful for differentiating those with conversion symptoms from those with organic disease. The other 4 did not comment on its utility.

The quality of the studies was generally poor. Only 6 studies were clearly of consecutive patients (Table 1) and only 6 studies were prospective (Table 1). The latter is important because a retrospective case-note review is unlikely to be a valid means of determining the presence of a clinical sign. Only 8 studies recorded the actual physical symptoms that led to the diagnosis of conversion symptoms (Table 1). This is also an important limitation, as it is much easier to detect la belle indifférence in a patient with paralysis than in an individual with non-epileptic seizures who is asymptomatic between episodes.

Only 2 studies clearly described what they meant by la belle indifférence (Reference Ebel and LohmannEbel & Lohmann, 1995) or referenced another description (Reference Gould, Miller and GoldbergGould et al, 1986), and none discussed any of the difficulties in making this judgement (see below). Although 1 study used a system of re-rating to improve the reliability of the clinical diagnosis of la belle indifférence (Reference BarnertBarnert, 1971), these data were not presented in the paper. Finally, in none of the studies were the investigators masked to the patient's diagnosis when assessing whether la belle indifférence was present.

DISCUSSION

The evidence from the published literature suggests that la belle indifférence is not a useful clinical sign for distinguishing between conversion symptoms and organic disease. The quality of the published studies was poor, many were retrospective, many provided an incomplete description of the patients’ symptoms, and none used operational criteria and masked ratings to assess whether la belle indifférence was present.

Limitations

The conclusions of this review must be qualified by the limitations inherent in the studies that it included. In addition, there were limitations in the methodology used for the systematic review. First, we only included studies that had been published since 1965. To our knowledge, no large relevant studies were published before that date. Second, the total number of patients in the review is small. Third, some of the patients who were included may have been wrongly diagnosed, although this is unlikely to be a major factor, as a systematic review found the overall rate of misdiagnosis of conversion disorder to be only 4% since 1970 (Reference Stone, Smyth and CarsonStone et al, 2005).

Meanings of la belle indifférence

The term la belle indifférence seems to have gained popularity after Freud used it to describe ‘Elizabeth von R’ in Studies on Hysteria (Reference Breuer and FreudBreuer & Freud, 1895). Freud later attributed the term to Charcot (Reference Freud and JonesFreud, 1915), which suggests that it may have been widely used from the end of the nineteenth century onwards. Janet (Reference Janet1907) briefly mentions indifference to both sensory loss and paralysis in his book, The Major Symptoms of Hysteria, but does not appear to use the term ‘la belle indifférence’. We could not find the term in any of the other well-known books about hysteria published at that time, including Charcot's translated lectures (Reference SkeySkey, 1867; Reference CharcotCharcot, 1889; Reference SavillSavill, 1909; Reference FoxFox, 1913), although indifference to areas of anaesthesia on examination was mentioned in many of those texts (see below). Thus, if the term was used clinically at that time, it was not deemed sufficiently important to be included in many texts about hysteria. It appeared with more regularity towards the middle of the twentieth century, predominantly in the psychoanalytical literature, before it achieved more widespread usage.

In tracing the history of the term ‘la belle indifférence’, it is clear that it has had more than one meaning since it was first used. We summarise these below.

Hysterical ‘stigmata’ or sensory signs of which the patient is unaware

The commonest description of indifference in the early literature related to the discovery of sensory signs or ‘stigmata’ of which the patient was unaware. Janet expressed this common clinical observation as follows:

This absence of objective disturbances is mostly accompanied by a very curious subjective symptom; namely, the indifference of the patient. When you watch a hysterical patient for the firsttime, or when you study patients coming from the country, who have not yet been examined by specialists, you will find, like ourselves, that, without suffering from it and without suspecting it, they have the deepest and most extensive anaesthesia…. Charcot has often insisted on this point and shown that many patients are much surprised when you reveal to them their insensibility.

(Reference JanetJanet, 1907)

Charcot and Janet described ‘hysterical stigmata’ such as hemisensory disturbance, ipsilateral constricted visual fields and reduced hearing, which were characteristically noticed on examination but not reported by the patient (who nevertheless did report other distressing symptoms). Centuries earlier, similar sensory ‘stigmata’ were used as evidence of witchcraft. This clinical phenomenon continues to be seen frequently and is recognised by neurologists as functional or psychogenic (Reference TothToth, 2003). However, lack of awareness of sensory disturbance is distinct from the serene indifference to actual disability that is suggested by contemporary descriptions of la belle indifférence.

Conversion of distress

The classic psychoanalytical interpretation of la belle indifférence is that it is evidence that an intrapsychic conflict has been converted and kept from its unacceptable conscious expression by the production of a physical symptom–so-called primary gain. Freud was the first to admit that this process of conversion was not always complete. However, when it is present la belle indifférence appears to represent physical evidence of the conversion process at work, and could be seen as potent evidence of its truth (one reason, perhaps, why it has been such a celebrated sign; Reference AbseAbse, 1966). As psychodynamic theory has progressed, more complex hypotheses have arisen to challenge this rather ‘hydraulic’ model of conversion (Reference ChodoffChodoff, 1954; Reference Greenberg and MitchellGreenberg & Mitchell, 1983). For example, Merskey (Reference Merskey1995) suggested that some patients may simply be relieved that they have escaped a more difficult problem in their life by becoming ill. However, the simple conversion model is still the most well known, perhaps because of the persistence of the term ‘conversion disorder’.

The simple conversion hypothesis is at odds with what is known about the frequency of psychiatric disorder and emotional distress in patients with conversion symptoms. Depression and anxiety are reported in 20–50% of patients with conversion symptoms (Reference Wilson-Barnett and TrimbleWilson-Barnett & Trimble, 1985; Reference Lecompte and ClaraLecompte & Clara, 1987; Reference Crimlisk, Bhatia and CopeCrimlisk et al, 1998). In addition, these patients invariably come to medical attention because they are distressed by their symptoms. These observations do not necessarily negate the conversion hypothesis. However, it must now compete with or accommodate other theoretical developments in this area, including the advances in cognitive neuropsychology and neurobiology discussed below (Reference SpenceSpence, 1999; Reference Halligan, Bass and MarshallHalligan et al, 2001; Reference BrownBrown, 2004).

Alternative explanations for apparent indifference

‘Putting on a brave face’ to avoid a psychiatric diagnosis

Freud's first use of the term la belle indifférence–to describe his patient Elizabeth von R in Studies on Hysteria–implies not so much a denial of disability that is obvious to everyone else, as ‘putting a brave face on things’.

She seemed intelligent and mentally normal and bore her troubles, which interfered with her social life and pleasures, with a cheerful air–the belle indifférence of a hysteric, I could not help thinking.

(Reference Breuer and FreudBreuer & Freud, 1895)

Patients with physical symptoms that cannot be explained by organic disease commonly combine clear distress about their physical symptoms with apparent resilience and cheerfulness. However, such cheerfulness is often easy to expose as superficial and as a ‘mask’ for the depression or anxiety that is identified by a more searching interview. In many cases, strenuous efforts at cheerfulness may simply reflect a desire by patients not to see themselves or be labelled by others as ‘depressed’ or ‘psychiatric’ cases. The following anonymised case from our own recent practice illustrates this:

A young woman had an attack characterised by panic with prominent dissociation, unresponsiveness and limb shaking during venepuncture the day after a surgical procedure. After a period of drowsiness she was found to have a marked right hemiparesis. Investigations to search for a neurological cause of her symptoms were negative and there were positive clinical features in favour of a diagnosis of conversion disorder, including a tubular visual field and strongly positive Hoover's sign on the affected side. The referring doctors commented on her affect, which was recorded in the notes as ‘unconcerned’, ‘unusually cheerful'and ‘indifferent’. Nursing staff agreed that this was her consistent affect. At interview the patient smiled frequently and did indeed appear unworried by her hemiparesis, even though she had no movement in her right arm and was unable to walk. After 20 min of interviewing, the patient was asked about her apparently cheerful demeanour. ‘Is this really how you are feeling about things or do you think you might be“putting a brave face on things”?’ The patient burst into tears and admitted being terrified both by her symptoms and by the possibility that some one was going to think she had ‘gone crazy’.

Patients often view psychiatric labels for physical symptoms as an implication that the symptoms are fabricated, imagined or relate to ‘going mad’ (Reference Stone, Wojcik and DurranceStone et al, 2002a ). In addition, patients with conversion symptoms tend to express the conviction that an organic disease is responsible for those symptoms even more strongly than patients whose symptoms are actually a result of an organic disease (Reference Creed, Firth and TimolCreed et al, 1990; Reference Binzer, Eisemann and KullgrenBinzer et al, 1998). It is hardly surprising, therefore, that many patients with these symptoms may try hard not to appear like ‘psychiatric’ cases. Thus, superficial cheerfulness in the face of adversity in an attempt to avoid a psychiatric diagnosis is not the same as indifference to physical disability as implied by la belle indifférence.

Attentional impairment or‘absent-mindedness’

Another difficulty in the assessment of la belle indifférence is defining for how much of the time the indifference is present and whether it is present when the patient is specifically asked about their disability. For example, a patient may appear indifferent most of the time but be quite clearly concerned when asked about their paralysed leg.

Lasegue and Janet wrote about the ‘absent-mindedness’ of ‘hystericals’ (Reference JanetJanet, 1901). Lasegue considered it to be a core psychological feature related to ‘general preoccupation’. Janet described it as follows:

an exaggerated state of absent-mindedness, which is not momentary and is not the result of voluntary attention turned in one direction; it is a state of natural and perpetual absent-mindedness which prevents those persons from appreciating any other sensation except the one which for the time occupies their mind.

(Reference JanetJanet, 1901)

Such ‘absent-mindedness’ would not be calm acceptance of disability but simply a general diminution of attention masquerading as indifference. The findings of a neuropsychological study of patients with conversion disorder have provided some support for this attentional hypothesis (Reference Roelofs, van Galen and ElingRoelofs et al, 2003).

La belle indifférence as a marker of factitious disorder

One final possible explanation of la belle indifférence is that it is the affect of someone who knows that their symptoms are under conscious control and who is therefore not concerned about them. There are no data to support or refute this hypothesis.

La belle indifférence: a biological perspective

If we accept that la belle indifférence does sometimes occur in conversion disorder, are there plausible biological reasons why this may be so? Anosognosia (denial of hemiplegia) and anosodiaphoria (indifference to hemiplegia) are surprisingly common clinical features of hemispheric lesions, particularly right parietal stroke. In one study, anosognosia was found in 28% and anosodiaphoria in another 27% of 171 patients with right hemisphere stroke (Reference Stone, Halligan and GreenwoodStone et al, 1993). Many authors have suggested that this may tell us something about the biology of la belle indifférence in conversion disorder. Functional neuroimaging is certainly now being used to explore the neural correlates of ‘hysterical’ motor and sensory symptoms. For example, in one study the hypoactivation of the contralateral thalamus seen in patients with hemisensory conversion symptoms recovered when the symptoms resolved (Reference Vuilleumier, Chicherio and AssalVuilleumier et al, 2001). Perhaps similar dysfunction of parietal areas could lead to la belle indifférence. However, there are two problems with this. First, as we have already mentioned, the existence of la belle indifférence is under threat because of its poor definition and the potential for misdiagnosis. Second, part of this biological hypothesis of la belle indifférence has been based on the idea that conversion symptoms, like neglect, invariably lateralise to the left side of the body. Both a recent study (Reference Stone, Sharpe and CarsonStone et al, 2002b ) and an earlier systematic review (Reference JonesJones, 1908) found that there was little evidence to support this hypothesis, particularly when the symptom is paralysis.

An alternative but again unproven biological explanation for la belle indifférence is that patients with severe conversion symptoms have frontal hypoactivation (Reference Spence, Crimlisk and CopeSpence et al, 2000) that could potentially contribute to a syndrome of apathy and indifference.

Other clinical signs of conversion disorder

The survival of la belle indifférence as a clinical sign over the past century should also be viewed in the context of the other clinical signs of conversion disorder/hysteria, such as collapsing weakness and ‘midline splitting’ of sensory loss. These signs have rarely been assessed in clinical studies and often show poor reliability for the identification of conversion disorder when they are tested in this way (Reference Stone, Zeman and SharpeStone et al, 2002c ). Although some clinical signs, such as Hoover's sign for paralysis, have recently been shown in some small studies to be potentially more reliable (Reference Ziv, Djaldetti and ZoldanZiv et al, 1998), it is perhaps not surprising that, among such untested signs, la belle indifférence has survived unchallenged for so long.

Theoretical and clinical implications

It is not difficult to see why la belle indifférence has continued to be included as a feature of conversion disorder. First, it has a romantic history providing a link between modern practice and famous historical figures such as Charcot and Freud. Giving any clinical sign a memorable name tends to heighten its profile (and doing so in French perhaps heightens it even more). Second, it is consistent with beliefs about the conversion of emotional distress into physical symptoms, which despite the lack of evidence for them are widely held. Third, theories linking la belle indifférence to right hemisphere dysfunction may have promoted the survival of the concept in an era of biological psychiatry. Fourth, it is yet another untested clinical sign among other untested clinical signs for ‘hysteria’. Finally, clinicians may not always have considered the ‘differential diagnosis’ of an apparently indifferent state. In our experience, this is most commonly manifested as an apparently cheerful patient with disability who is actually distressed but who makes strenuous efforts to avoid providing possible evidence for those seeking to make a psychiatric diagnosis, and thus to avoid the stigma associated with the latter.

The findings of this systematic review do not support the use of la belle indifférence as a clinical sign for discriminating between conversion symptoms/hysteria and organic disease. The review also highlights the poor quality of the published studies that have addressed the subject, and raises questions about what la belle indifférence actually means. We conclude that further research is required to define and study apparent indifference, in particular looking for alternative explanations for this sign. Despite its attractive name, la belle indifférence should be abandoned as a clinical sign until both its definition and its utility have been clarified.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. The available evidence suggests that la belle indifférence does not discriminate between conversion symptoms/hysteria and symptoms of organic disease.

  2. La belle indifférence, defined as a lack of concern about symptoms, may be confused with other reasons for apparent indifference, most commonly strenuous efforts by a patient to appear cheerful so as to avoid being labelled as a ‘psychiatric case’.

  3. The use of the term should be abandoned until its definition and utility have been clarified.

LIMITATIONS

  1. The published studies of la belle indifférence are of poor methodological quality, are unmasked and lack operationalised criteria.

  2. The total number of patients included in the review is relatively small.

  3. Studies published before 1965 were not included in the review.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Steff Lewis for statistical advice. J.S. was funded by the Chief Scientist Office, Scotland.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

Abse, W. D. (1966) Hysteria and Related Mental Disorders. Bristol: John Wright.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn, revised) (DSM–IV–R). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Barnert, C. (1971) Conversion reactions and psychophysiologic disorders: a comparative study. Psychiatry in Medicine, 2, 205220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Binzer, M., Eisemann, M. & Kullgren, G. (1998) Illness behavior in the acute phase of motor disability in neurological disease and in conversion disorder: a comparative study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 44, 657666.Google Scholar
Breuer, J. E. & Freud, S. (1895) Studien über hysterie. Leipzig: Deuticke.Google Scholar
Brown, R. J. (2004) Psychological mechanisms of medically unexplained symptoms: an integrative conceptual model. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 793812.Google Scholar
Chabrol, H., Peresson, G. & Clanet, M. (1995) Lack of specificity of the traditional criteria of conversion disorders. European Psychiatry, 10, 317319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charcot, J. M. (1889) Clinical Lectures on Diseases of the Nervous System. London: New Sydenham Society.Google Scholar
Chodoff, P. (1954) A re-examination of some aspects of conversion hysteria. Psychiatry, 17, 7581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creed, F., Firth, D., Timol, M., et al (1990) Somatization and illness behaviour in a neurology ward. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 34, 427437.Google Scholar
Crimlisk, H. L., Bhatia, K., Cope, H., et al (1998) Slater revisited: 6-year follow-up study of patients with medically unexplained motor symptoms. BMJ, 316, 582586.Google Scholar
Dickes, R. A. (1974) Brief therapy of conversion reactions: an in-hospital technique. American Journal of Psychiatry, 131, 584586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ebel, H. & Lohmann, T. (1995) Clinical criteria for diagnosing conversion disorders. Neurology, Psychiatry and Brain Research, 3, 193200.Google Scholar
Fox, C. D. (1913) The Psychopathology of Hysteria. Boston, MA: Gorham Press.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1915) Repression. Republished (1948) in Collected Papers vol. IV (ed. and trans. Jones, E.). London: Hogarth Press & Institute of Psychoanalysis.Google Scholar
Gould, R., Miller, B. L., Goldberg, M. A., et al (1986) The validity of hysterical signs and symptoms. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174, 593597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, J. R. & Mitchell, S. A. (1983) Object Relations in Psychoanalytic Theory Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Halligan, P., Bass, C. & Marshall, J. C. (2001) Contemporary Approaches to the Science of Hysteria: Clinical and Theoretical Perspectives. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janet, P. (1901) The Mental State of Hystericals. New York: Putnams.Google Scholar
Janet, P. (1907) The Major Symptoms of Hysteria. London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, E. (1908) Le côte affecté par l'hémiplégie hysttérique. Revue Neurologique (Paris), 16, 193196.Google Scholar
Kapfhammer, H. P., Buchheim, P., Bove, D., et al (1992) Konverssionssymptome bei patienten im psychiatrischen konsiliardienst. Nervenarzt, 63, 527538.Google Scholar
Lecompte, D. & Clara, A. (1987) Associated psychopathologyin conversion patients without organic disease. Acta Psychiatrica Belgica, 87, 654661.Google Scholar
Lewis, W. C. & Berman, M. (1965) Studies of conversion hysteria. Archives of General Psychiatry, 13, 275282.Google Scholar
Merskey, H. (1995) The Analysis of Hysteria: Understanding Conversion and Dissociation (2nd edn). London: Gaskell.Google Scholar
Raskin, M., Talbott, J. A. & Meyerson, A. T. (1966) Diagnosis of conversion reactions. Predictive value of psychiatric criteria. JAMA, 197, 530534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reed, J. L. (1975) The diagnosis of ‘hysteria'. Psychological Medicine, 5, 1317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roelofs, K., van Galen, G. P., Eling, P., et al (2003) Endogenous and exogenous attention in patients with conversion disorders. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 733745.Google Scholar
Savill, T. D. (1909) Lectures on Hysteria and Allied Vasomotor Conditions. London: Glaisher.Google Scholar
Sharma, P. & Chaturvedi, S. K. (1995) Conversion disorder revisited. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 92, 301304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, M. & Barthel, R. P. (1986) Conversion disorders on a child psychiatry consultation service. Psychosomatics, 27, 201204.Google Scholar
Skey, F. C. (1867) Hysteria: Remote Causes of Disease in General. Treatment of Disease by Tonic Agency, Local or Surgical Forms of Hysteria, etc. London: Longmans.Google Scholar
Spence, S. A. (1999) Hysterical paralyses as disorders of action. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 4, 203226.Google Scholar
Spence, S. A., Crimlisk, H. L., Cope, H., et al (2000) Discrete neurophysiological correlates in prefrontal cortexduring hysterical and feigned disorder of movement. Lancet, 355, 12431244.Google Scholar
Stone, J., Wojcik, W., Durrance, D., et al (2002a) What should we say to patients with symptoms unexplained by disease? The ‘number needed to offend'. BMJ, 325, 14491450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stone, J., Sharpe, M., Carson, A., et al (2002b) Are functional motor and sensory symptoms really more frequent on the left? A systematic review. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 73, 578681.Google Scholar
Stone, J., Zeman, A. & Sharpe, M. (2002c) Functional weakness and sensory disturbance. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 73, 241245.Google Scholar
Stone, J., Smyth, R., Carson, A., et al (2005) Systematic review of misdiagnosis of conversion symptoms and ‘hysteria'. BMJ, 331, 989.Google Scholar
Stone, S. P., Halligan, P. W. & Greenwood, R. J. (1993) The incidence of neglect phenomena and related disorders in patients with an acute right or left hemisphere stroke. Age and Ageing, 22, 4652.Google Scholar
Toth, C. (2003) Hemisensory syndrome is associated with a low diagnostic yield and a nearly uniform benign prognosis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 74, 11131116.Google Scholar
Vuilleumier, P., Chicherio, C., Assal, F., et al (2001) Functional neuroanatomical correlates of hysterical sensorimotor loss. Brain, 124, 10771090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstein, E. A. & Lyerly, O. G. (1966) Conversion hysteria following brain injury. Archives of Neurology, 15, 545548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstein, E. A., Eck, R. A. & Lyerly, O. G. (1969) Conversion hysteria in Appalachia. Psychiatry, 32, 334341.Google Scholar
Wilson-Barnett, J. & Trimble, M. R. (1985) An investigation of hysteria using the Illness Behaviour Questionnaire. British Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 601608.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1992) International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (10th revision) (ICD–10). Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
Ziv, I., Djaldetti, R., Zoldan, Y., et al (1998) Diagnosis of ‘non-organic’ limb paresis by anovel objective motor assessment: the quantitative Hoover's test. Journal of Neurology, 245, 797802.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Frequency ofla belle indifférence. Each point represents an individual study in the review, and the size of the point is related to the number of the patients in the study. The lines represent 95% binomial exact confidence intervals. ▒ Conversion symptoms/hysteria (n=356); ░ organic disease (n=157).

Figure 1

Table 1 Studies reporting la belle indifférence in patients with conversion symptoms and organic disease

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.