Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T10:52:08.283Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Relationship between disabling fatigue and depression in children

Genetic study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Tom A. Fowler*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, Cardiff University, UK
Frances Rice
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, Cardiff University, UK
Anita Thapar
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, Cardiff University, UK
Anne Farmer
Affiliation:
MRC Social, Genetic, Developmental Psychiatric Research Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, London, UK
*
Dr Tom Fowler, Section of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Department of Psychological Medicine, Cardiff University, Heath Park, Cardiff, Wales CF14 4XN, UK. Email: fowlerta@cardiff.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Medically unexplained disabling fatigue in young people is familial and frequently associated with depressed mood.

Aims

To examine the degree of sharing of genetic and environmental influences on the symptoms of depression and fatigue in this age group.

Method

The parents of twins aged 8–17 years, derived from a population-based register, completed a questionnaire regarding lifetime-ever disabling fatigue in both twins. Twins aged 11 years or over completed the Mood and Feelings Questionnaire. The genetic and environmental influences on fatigue and the relationship with depression were examined using bivariate genetic analysis.

Results

Parent-rated data were obtained for 1468 twin pairs (65%) and self-rated data from 930 older twin pairs (58%). Bivariate analysis of fatigue and depression suggested that genetic and environmental influences on disabling fatigue were mainly specific to fatigue.

Conclusions

Unexplained disabling fatigue in childhood is substantially familial and has mainly an independent aetiology from depression.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2006 

Chronic fatigue in young people can be disabling and is the main illness-related reason for prolonged absence from school (Reference Dowsett and ColbyDowsett & Colby, 1997). Despite this, to date there has only been one study that has specifically examined the genetic aetiology of disabling fatigue in children (Reference Farmer, Scourfield and MartinFarmer et al, 1999). Symptoms of disabling fatigue and depression commonly co-occur (Reference MarshallMarshall, 1999; Reference Garralda and RangelGarralda & Rangel, 2002), and studies have shown that children suffering from chronic fatigue syndrome have significantly higher rates of depression than those with other chronic illness (Reference Walford, Nelson and McCluskeyWalford et al, 1993; Reference Carter, Kronenberger and EdwardsCarter et al, 1999; Reference Brace, Scott Smith and McCauleyBrace et al, 2000). Consequently, it is important to examine whether the co-occurrence of these two disorders arises because of shared genetic and/or environmental influences. We set out to examine the genetic and environmental aetiology of disabling fatigue and its relationship with depressive symptoms, using a twin study design.

METHOD

Participants

The participants were identified from the Cardiff Study of All Wales and North West of England Twins (CASTANET), a population-based twin register covering two geographical areas, South Wales (Reference Scourfield, Martin and LewisScourfield et al, 1999) and nine health districts in Greater Manchester and Lancashire (Reference Thapar, Harrington and RossThapar et al, 2000). Earlier studies have shown the register to be representative of the local population (Reference Thapar and McGuffinThapar & McGuffin, 1994; Reference Scourfield, Martin and LewisScourfield et al, 1999; Reference Thapar, Harrington and RossThapar et al, 2000).

The study design included some twin pairs (n=439) from South Wales who participated in the earlier study on fatigue by Farmer and colleagues (Reference Farmer, Scourfield and Martin1999). However, our analyses were based on data obtained from a population-based extended sample of twins, the majority of whom had not previously been studied, and the focus is on comorbidity with depression.

Initially 2259 twin pairs, aged 8-17 years, were identified from the register if their current addresses could be verified through a British Telecommunications telephone database and/or the electoral role. Of these, 656 twin pairs were aged between 8 and 10 years and 1603 were aged between 11 and 17 years. The general practitioners (GPs) of all the twins were first approached to check for any contraindications to contact by the study team. Twins who had emigrated and pairs where one twin had died or suffered from a serious illness or was no longer living in the same household were excluded (n=34). Twin families who did not wish to take part were also excluded (n=19).

Measures

The parents of all twins were sent questionnaires by post, with three reminders. As there is evidence that self-reports of depression are not valid in children under the age of 11 years (Reference Meltzer, Gatward and GoodmanMeltzer et al, 2000), self-ratings were obtained for children over the age of 11 years (adolescents) only. Zygosity was assigned using a standard twin similarity questionnaire that has been shown to have over 90% accuracy in distinguishing between monozygotic and dizygotic twins (Reference Cohen, Dibble and GraweCohen et al, 1975). A questionnaire measure of environmental sharing was also included (Reference Loehlin and NicholsLoehlin & Nichols, 1976). This consisted of items asking, for example, whether the twins dressed the same or shared the same friends, and was used to test the equal environment assumption that underlies twin analyses.

Fatigue was rated by asking parents if there had ever been a period of fatigue lasting several days or weeks where a twin felt very tired and was unable to go to school, college, work or out. If the response to this was ‘yes’, parents were asked to indicate how long the fatigue had lasted on a 5-point scale:

  1. 1. a few days of disabling fatigue;

  2. 2. more than 1 week but less than 1 month of disabling fatigue;

  3. 3. more than 1 month but less than 3 months of disabling fatigue;

  4. 4. more than 3 months but less than 6 months of disabling fatigue;

  5. 5. 6 months or more of disabling fatigue.

This is the measure of disabling fatigue used by Farmer et al (Reference Farmer, Scourfield and Martin1999). All analyses were conducted using parental report of fatigue in the twins.

With regard to the validity of the measure of disabling fatigue, the parents of all individuals for whom there had been reported fatigue of more than 1 week were asked to complete a telephone interview about the period of fatigue. Questions were asked about the duration of the fatigue and associated characteristics, including those symptoms also necessary for a diagnosis of chronic fatigue syndrome (Reference Fukuda, Straus and HickieFukuda et al, 1994). The nature and degree of impairment associated with the fatigue were assessed by enquiring about whether the twin needed to rest for at least 1 h daily specifically because of fatigue, and whether there was interference with school attendance and/or usual leisure activities and with family and peer relationships. To be classed as having disabling fatigue, the twin was required to need to rest for at least 1 h daily and for there to be a report of some interference in at least one of these life areas. A more detailed description of the composition of this interview is reported elsewhere (Reference Farmer, Fowler and ScourfieldFarmer et al, 2004; Reference Fowler, Duthie and ThaparFowler et al, 2005).

Of those parents who agreed to take part (78%), all confirmed the questionnaire report of a period of disabling fatigue that lasted at least 1 week and was disabling and impairing in at least one of the following areas: family interactions, social interactions and school. The associated demographic characteristics of this group resembled those of clinical samples of children with chronic fatigue syndrome (Reference Farmer, Fowler and ScourfieldFarmer et al, 2004; Reference Fowler, Duthie and ThaparFowler et al, 2005). This suggests that the questions on fatigue were assessing disabling fatigue in children and adolescents.

Because of the effects of categorical analysis, and the rarity of disabling fatigue when defined in its more extreme state, analysis was only undertaken for disabling fatigue of more than 1 week (short duration of fatigue) and for disabling fatigue of more than 1 month (prolonged fatigue). These cut points had the advantage of having been used previously in behavioural genetic research on fatigue in both children (Reference Farmer, Scourfield and MartinFarmer et al, 1999) and adults (Reference Hickie, Kirk and MartinHickie et al, 1999; Reference Sullivan, Kovalenko and YorkSullivan et al, 2003). Furthermore, Fukuda and colleagues (Reference Fukuda, Straus and Hickie1994), when proposing a framework for examining fatigue as a disorder (which includes the definition of chronic fatigue syndrome), also proposed a broad category of disabling fatigue of greater than 1 month (prolonged fatigue). There is evidence from the interview data that using broader definitions for disabling fatigue is still clinically relevant. No significant difference in terms of impairment, comorbidity with depression, male: female ratio, age of onset and days per week affected was found between individuals who just fulfilled the broader definitions of disabling fatigue and those who fulfilled the criteria for chronic fatigue syndrome (Reference Fowler, Duthie and ThaparFowler et al, 2005).

Self-reports of depressive symptoms were used and depression was rated using the long version of the Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (MFQ; Reference Costello and AngoldCostello & Angold, 1988). The MFQ is a 34-item questionnaire that has been well validated in community-based samples (Reference Cooper and GoodyerCooper & Goodyer, 1993; Reference Wood, Kroll and MooreWood et al, 1995). Each item is rated on a three-point scale (0, never; 1, sometimes; 2 often) and a total score is obtained by summing each item score. Individuals were asked to score each item with regard to how they had been feeling in the previous 3 months. No reliability or validity data for the MFQ were collected on this sample. However, previous reports from the CASTANET cohort have shown validity of the MFQ with diagnoses of depressive disorder derived from the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment (CAPA; Reference Thapar and McGuffinThapar & McGuffin, 1998) and stability of MFQ symptoms over a 3-year period (Pearson's r=0.5) (Reference Scourfield, Rice and ThaparScourfield et al, 2003).

Genetic analyses

Univariate models

A liability threshold model of disabling fatigue was assumed whereby the liability to disabling fatigue is considered as normally, or approximately normally, distributed in the population, and only those above a certain threshold of liability display the symptoms of fatigue (Reference FalconerFalconer, 1965). Tetrochoric correlations and concordance rates were calculated initially to assess genetic and environmental influences on disabling fatigue as defined by increasingly more stringent periods of disabling fatigue. Monozygotic twins share 100% of their genes in common, whereas dizygotic twins share on average 50%. Thus, for genetically influenced disorders, we would expect higher monozygotic than dizygotic correlations. Standard univariate analysis using contingency tables and the maximum likelihood fit function, assuming bivariate normal liability, were undertaken using the structural equation modelling programme Mx (Reference NealeNeale, 1999). This estimated the contribution of additive genetic (A), shared environment (C) and non-shared environment (E) to the variance of liability of disabling fatigue. This analysis was further extended by fitting a general-effects gender-limitation model, a common-effects gender-limitation model and gender-homogeneity model to test for possible gender differences in aetiology (Reference Neale and CardonNeale & Cardon, 1992). By comparing a general-effects gender-limitation model to a common-effects gender-limitation model it is possible to test whether there are distinct genetic influences in males and females. By comparing the common-effects gender-limitation model to a gender-homogeneity model (which equates the separate male and female estimates), it is possible to test whether there are significant differences in the magnitude of the influences on the underlying aetiology of the disorder in males and females.

Bivariate models examining fatigue and depression

We set out to examine whether there are distinct genetic and environmental influences on disabling fatigue that are separate from those contributing to depression. A bivariate Cholesky decomposition allows this to be tested and has been used to do so in adults with disabling fatigue (Reference Hickie, Kirk and MartinHickie et al, 1999). By placing depression first in the analysis, the first latent factors A1, C1 and E1 (Fig. 1) load on both variables, and the second latent factors A2, C2 and E2 load specifically on disabling fatigue. This allows the examination of the extent to which a common set of genetic and environmental factors account for the covariation between the two phenotypes: disabling fatigue and depression.

Fig. 1 Path diagram showing latent genetic and environmental influences on twin-rated depression and parent-rated short-duration fatigue. A, additive genetic; C, shared environment; E, non-shared environment; *significant at P=0.05. Standardised path coefficients are provided. These must be squared to obtain proportions of variance of the measured variable accounted for the latent variable.

In this analysis, depression was also treated as a continuum of liability. As this type of analysis requires a certain number of twins in each category, the depression scores were grouped into six categories, as this was the maximum number of equally sized categories that ensured at least one individual in each category. As previous analysis of the depression data (Reference Rice, Harold and ThaparRice et al, 2002) demonstrated the importance of shared environment, it was hypothesised that shared environmental factors might be important in the covariation between disabling fatigue and depression. However, in bivariate analyses where the same informant rates both variables, shared method variance could explain a finding of shared environmental factors contributing to covariance (Reference Rutter, Pickles and MurrayRutter et al, 2001). To avoid such a confound, bivariate analysis was undertaken across rater using parental ratings of disabling fatigue and twin self-ratings of depression.

For the bivariate analysis, a raw data analysis approach was taken. This allows information from twin pairs with missing data to be included in the analysis by estimating the missing values. This is particularly useful in multivariate analysis, where missing values can substantially reduce the sample size. Conventional tests of fit are not appropriate for this type of analysis; however, as the relative difference in the fit of minus twice the log-likelihood between submodels is distributed as χ2, it is possible to compare the bivariate model to a perfect-fitting model in which there are no constraints. This perfect-fitting model is also referred to as a saturated model. If the bivariate model is not significantly different from this saturated model, it represents a good fit of the data (Reference Rijsdijk, Snieder and OrmelRijsdijk et al, 2003).

RESULTS

Response rate and sample characteristics

Parent-rated questionnaires were returned for 1468 pairs of twins, representing a response rate of 65% of these returned questionnaires, 1052 were for twins aged between 11 and 17 years. Of the 1603 twins aged between 11 and 17 years, who were asked also to complete self-report questionnaires, 930 (58%) returned them. Parent-report suggested 129 individuals (monozygotic=53, dizygotic=73, zygosity unassigned=3) could be categorised as suffering from short duration of fatigue and 73 individuals (monozygotic=26, dizygotic=45, zygosity unassigned=2) from prolonged fatigue. The mean self-rated MFQ scores for the twins was 11.26 (s.d.=9.89) for monozygotic twins and 11.90 (s.d.=10.28) for dizygotic twins (Reference Rice, Harold and ThaparRice et al, 2002).

Of the individuals for whom parental interview data were available (i.e. 77% of all individuals classed as suffering from disabling fatigue in the genetic analysis), approximately two-thirds were female (67%), the average age of onset for the fatigue was 11 years and 3 months and, although the majority did not fulfil the duration criteria for chronic fatigue syndrome, 80% had four or more associated symptoms also required for that diagnosis (Reference Fukuda, Straus and HickieFukuda et al, 1994). There was no significant difference in any of these characteristics between those who were classed as suffering from short-duration fatigue and those classed as suffering from prolonged fatigue. There was a significant difference (t=-1.97, P=0.05) between MFQ scores for individuals categorised as suffering from short-duration fatigue only (mean=15.31, s.d.=10.16) and those who suffered from prolonged fatigue (mean=20.54, s.d.=14.05).

There were no significant differences between those who replied and those who did not, in terms of ecological socio-demographic indices that could be obtained by comparing the postcodes of responders and non-responders (t=1.71, P=0.09). Social class information was available only for the Manchester twins. For these twins, the percentage of families from the various social class groups (I, II, III, IV, V) were in keeping with those expected from the Manchester population (Public Health Common Data Set, 1993; Reference Rice, Harold and ThaparRice et al, 2002).

Environmental sharing

Monozygotic twins scored significantly higher on the environmental sharing (t=17.62, P=0.01), but this did not account for the monozygotic similarity in short-duration fatigue (OR=0.935, 95% CI 0.747-1.169, P=0.553) or prolonged fatigue (OR=0.946, 95% CI 0.670-1.336, P=0.752), nor did it account for the depression scores (R2=0.003, F=2.866, P=0.091) (Reference Rice, Harold and ThaparRice et al, 2002).

Genetic analysis

Univariate analysis

The concordance rates for short-duration fatigue were monozygotic=35%, dizygotic=14%; the tetrachoric correlations were monozygotic=0.66 (95% CI 0.45-0.83), dizygotic=0.33 (95% CI 0.07-0.56). For prolonged fatigue the concordance rates were monozygotic=25%, dizygotic=19%; the tetrachoric correlations were monozygotic=0.62 (95% CI 0.27-0.85) dizygotic=0.50 (95% CI 0.20-0.72).

The tetrachoric correlations for the data suggest that there is an important additive genetic component (A) for short-duration fatigue. The similar monozygotic and dizygotic tetrachoric correlation coefficients for prolonged fatigue suggest an increase in the importance of the shared environmental contribution. This is confirmed by the model fitting. ACE models were fitted to the data, as shown in Table 1; full models with confidence intervals are reported. For short-duration fatigue, genetic factors contributed the most and the shared environmental contribution was estimated at zero. For prolonged fatigue the full ACE model showed mainly environmental influences but modest genetic variance (25%). There was no significant difference between the general gender-limitation model and the common gender-limitation model for either short-duration fatigue (Δχ2 1=0.01) or prolonged fatigue (Δχ2 1=0.19), suggesting no gender-specific genetic influences. Neither was there a significant difference between the common gender-limitation models and the gender-homogeneity models (short-duration fatigue, Δχ2 3 0.90; prolonged fatigue, Δχ2 3=4.28), suggesting no gender differences in the magnitude of the genetic and environmental influences.

Table 1 Univariate genetic model-fitting for parent-rated short-duration fatigue and prolonged fatigue using full and nested models

Threshold of disabling fatigue a2 (95% CI) c2 (95% CI) e2 (95% CI) χ2 d.f. P AIC
Short-duration fatigue
   ACE model 0.67 (0.04–0.82) 0.00 (0.00–0.51) 0.33 (0.18–0.55) 1.10 2 0.58 –2.90
   AE model 0.67 (0.47–0.82) [0] 0.33 (0.18–0.53) 1.10 3 0.78 –4.90
   CE model [0] 0.50 (0.33–0.65) 0.50 (0.35–0.67) 5.43 3 0.14 –0.58
   E model [0] [0] 1.00 (1.00–1.00) 34.80 4 0.00 26.80
Prolonged fatigue
   ACE model 0.25 (0.00–0.84) 0.37 (0.00–0.71) 0.38 (0.15–0.66) 1.41 2 0.49 –2.59
   AE model 0.69 (0.41–0.87) [0] 0.31 (0.13–0.59) 2.74 3 0.43 –3.27
   CE model [0] 0.55 (0.33–0.72) 0.45 (0.28–0.67) 1.77 3 0.62 –4.22
   E model [0] [0] 1.00 (1.00–1.00) 22.43 4 0.00 14.43

A, additive genetic; C, shared environment; E, non-shared environment; a2, additive genetic variance; c2 shared environmental variance; e2, non-shared environmental variance; [0], parameter set at zero; d.f. degrees of freedom; AIC, Akaike's information criterion (Reference AkaikeAkaike, 1987)

Bivariate analysis

As previous work suggests a gender difference for twin-rated depression (Reference Rice, Harold and ThaparRice et al, 2002) MFQ scores were first standardised according to gender.

As statistical power is affected by the categorical data and the relatively small number of individuals with disabling fatigue, full models are reported with confidence intervals.

Short-duration fatigue

The phenotypic correlation between short-duration fatigue and depression was relatively small (polychoric correlation=0.28). The Cholesky decomposition for three sources of variance-additive genetics (A), shared environment (C) and non-shared environment (E) (Fig. 1), when compared with a saturated model was not significantly different (Δχ2 5=3.87) suggesting the model was a good fit. The majority of the variance in disabling fatigue appeared to be distinct from that of depression (Table 2, Fig. 1) with only 22% of the variance of disabling fatigue explained by factors common to both phenotypes. Of that 22%, shared environment accounted for the largest proportion of variance (13%), with genetic factors explaining only 8%.

Table 2 Bivariate genetic model-fitting for twin-rated depression and parent-rated disabling fatigue using full models

Threshold of disabling fatigue Phenotype A1 (95% CI) C1 (95% CI) E1 (95% CI) A2 (95% CI) C2 (95% CI) E2 (95% CI) Difference in fit from saturated model Δχ2 5
Short-duration fatigue Depression 0.59 (0.37–0.66) * 0.00 (0.00–0.17) 0.41 (0.34–0.50) * 3.87
Disabling fatigue 0.08 (0.00–0.21) 0.13 (0.00–0.63) 0.01 (0.00–0.07) 0.52 (0.00–0.80) 0.00 (0.00–0.63) 0.26 (0.11–0.49) *
Prolonged fatigue Depression 0.59 (0.37–0.63) * 0.00 (0.00–0.17) 0.41 (0.34–0.49) * 7.01
Disabling fatigue 0.11 (0.00–0.28) 0.34 (0.00–0.75) 0.01 (0.00–0.09) 0.30 (0.00–0.81) 0.00 (0.00–0.70) 0.24 (0.09–0.51) *

Standardised parameter estimates for the sources of variation in liability to depression and disabling fatigue are provided.

A1, A2, additive genetic variance; C1, C2, shared environmental variance; E1, E2, non-shared environmental variance

* P < 0.05

Prolonged fatigue

The phenotypic correlation between prolonged fatigue and depression (polychoric correlation=0.30) was slightly higher than that between short-duration fatigue and depression. For disabling fatigue defined as greater than 1 month, a much higher percentage of the variance was explained by factors common to both disabling fatigue and depression (46%). The fit of the model was not significantly worse than the saturated model (Δχ2 5=7.01). Full models with confidence intervals are reported (Table 2, Fig. 2). Just over half of the variance was specific to disabling fatigue (54%). Of the remaining variance, shared environment accounted for the largest proportion (36%), with genetic factors accounting for 28%.

Fig. 2 Path diagram showing latent genetic and environmental influences on twin-rated depression and parent-rated prolonged fatigue. A, additive genetic; C, shared environment; E, non-shared environment; *significant at P=0.05. Standardised path coefficients are provided. These must be squared to obtain proportions of variance of the measured variable accounted for the latent variable.

Cross-rater analyses were undertaken to control for the possibility of shared rater effect. Additional within-rater bivariate analysis of parent-rated fatigue and parentrated depression showed that the majority of the variance for both short-duration fatigue and prolonged fatigue was specific to the fatigue (76% and 63% respectively). The remaining variance was accounted for by environmental factors (both shared and non-shared), with genetic factors accounting for a negligible amount (<1%).

DISCUSSION

This investigation has examined the genetic and environmental influences on disabling fatigue, using a community sample of 2259 twin pairs aged 8-17 years. It has also examined the overlap between disabling fatigue and depression in those twin pairs (1052) aged 11-17 years.

The genetic aetiology of disabling fatigue

The results suggest that the aetiology of short-duration fatigue in children and adolescents is heritable (67%). In addition, the present results suggest that prolonged fatigue is familial. These findings are in accordance with earlier findings on a smaller sample of twins reported by Farmer and colleagues (Reference Farmer, Scourfield and Martin1999), which suggested disabling fatigue of greater than 1 week and disabling fatigue of greater than 1 month were both familial.

The results of the genetic analysis suggest that shared environment plays a more important role in prolonged fatigue than in the genetic aetiology of short-duration fatigue. This raises the question as to why there is an increase in the influence of shared environment in prolonged fatigue. It may be that individuals who are classed as experiencing a short period of fatigue have a propensity to being fatigued and that this is a genetically influenced trait. On the other hand, those with prolonged fatigue more resemble individuals with a specific disorder that may have a greater variety of aetiological factors.

Gender differences in aetiology

Interestingly, no significant differences in the magnitude of genetic and environmental effects between the genders were found. This is despite reports of clinical samples of chronic fatigue syndrome (Reference MarshallMarshall, 1999; Reference Garralda and RangelGarralda & Rangel, 2002), in which there is generally a preponderance of females, which might suggest aetiological differences between males and females. However, in view of the relatively large confidence intervals reported from the univariate analysis, these results should be interpreted with caution.

The relationship between fatigue and depression

The bivariate analyses suggest that for the more extreme definition of disabling fatigue there are greater shared aetiological influences with depression. However, for both short-duration fatigue and prolonged fatigue, the majority of the genetic and environmental variance is specific to disabling fatigue and distinct from depression. This suggests that fatigued states in children should be considered as valid entities in their own right and not as variants of depression. Hickie et al (Reference Hickie, Kirk and Martin1999) also found evidence that implies depression has a distinct aetiology from disabling fatigue of greater than 1 month, in a sample consisting of twins over the age of 50 years. Thus, a similar pattern for the aetiology of chronic fatigue has been found at both extremes of the life span. Williamson et al (Reference Williamson, Purcell and Sterne2005), in an analysis of sibling pairs (aged 20-55 years) also suggested that a substantial proportion of both familial and environmental aspects of the aetiology of fatigue were specific to fatigue.

Perhaps one of the most interesting findings was that, of the aetiology common to both phenotypes, shared environment had the greatest influence on disabling fatigue lasting at least 1 month. In standard bivariate genetic analysis, shared-method variance owing to the same rater reporting on both phenotypes may be partitioned into shared environmental effects. What is so striking in our study is that, since the analysis was cross-rater, this could not explain the shared environmental findings. As interview data had been collected on parental opinion as to the causes of the disabling fatigue and on parental report of GPs' explanation as to the cause of the disabling fatigue, this was also examined. Viral infection was the most common explanation of the fatigue, given by GPs when consulted. Viral infections were also an important cause according to parents, as was stress caused by a negative life event (Reference Farmer, Fowler and ScourfieldFarmer et al, 2004). These negative life events consisted of such things as parents splitting up, parental illness or death, school exams, increased workload at school or moving schools: life events that were probably shared by both twins. It should, however, be noted that these events may impact differently on each twin. Although viral infections are not thought to be necessary or sufficient to cause disabling fatigue, they have been implicated as possible triggers (Reference Garralda and RangelGarralda & Rangel, 2002). Individuals with chronic fatigue syndrome also appear to show higher levels of stressful life events just before the onset of the disorder (Reference Hatcher and HouseHatcher & House, 2003). Considering that these are also risk factors for depression in young people (e.g., Reference Goodyer, Cooper and VizeGoodyer et al, 1993; Reference Monroe, Rohde and SeeleyMonroe et al, 1999), it is possible that the increase in the importance of shared environment may be owing to the impact of viral illness and shared life events on making both twins more vulnerable to prolonged fatigue and depression.

Seeing that the results from the genetic analysis suggest that shared environment may be more important in people with longer periods of fatigue, future research should concentrate on both environmental and genetic risk factors. In view of the possible importance of shared life events and viral infections in explaining the relationship between fatigue and depression, both longitudinal and prospective studies examining these factors are called for, it also seems plausible that, in part, the high levels of depression seen may be the person's response to a disabling disorder. The most appropriate method with which to test this would be longitudinal genetically sensitive studies. These would examine the direction of the relationship between fatigue and depression and whether the relationship is owing to shared genetic and environmental risk factors.

In summary, disabling fatigue and depression co-occur but appear to have distinct genetic and environmental aetiologies.

Limitations

There are a number of limitations to this study. First, for the bivariate analysis, the measure of disabling fatigue was lifetime-ever, whereas the MFQ measured depressive symptoms in the previous 3 months. This is likely to have reduced the comorbidity between depression symptoms and disabling fatigue, and may have led to an underestimate of shared aetiological factors. However, there is longitudinal evidence from within the twin sample that levels of depressive symptoms are relatively stable over time, with a correlation of 0.5 between depression scores collected 3 years previously and the present wave of data collection (Reference Scourfield, Rice and ThaparScourfield et al, 2003). Furthermore, over 40% of parents interviewed reported that their child's fatigue was ongoing at the time of interview (and therefore current). As the depression section of the parent version of the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment (CAPA; Reference Angold and CostelloAngold & Costello, 2000) was also undertaken, a measure of depressive symptoms during the disabling fatigue was available. There was no significant difference in the correlation coefficients between the twins MFQ scores and the number of depressive symptoms during the period of fatigue for those with ongoing disabling fatigue (r=0.44) and those who previously suffered from disabling fatigue (r=0.45) (Fisher z=0.47, P=0.33). The small numbers available precluded any model fitting for those individuals currently fatigued, but cross-twin cross-trait polychoric correlations were approximately the same for those currently fatigued and those previously suffering from fatigue. Although it is almost certain that the differences in time scale of the measures for disabling fatigue and depressive symptoms will lead to an underestimate of the phenotypic correlation, the evidence from the interview data suggests that this is likely to have been a small effect.

Second, the measure of disabling fatigue was based on self-report questionnaire data and was therefore fairly broad and imprecise. In what ways the measure relates to the clinical definition of chronic fatigue syndrome remains uncertain, although the interview data do suggest that it represents a fairly good indication of disabling fatigue.

Third, the study represents a partial resampling of those contacted originally in the Farmer et al (Reference Farmer, Scourfield and Martin1999) paper and therefore is not entirely independent. Nonetheless, it does represent a new wave of data collection, and the twin register has been markedly increased by combining the original South Wales register with the Greater Manchester twin register. The results from a reanalysis of univariate data excluding the twins previously sampled gave broadly the same results but with larger confidence intervals. This study has also undertaken further analyses, namely the examination of the genetic contribution to gender differences in fatigue and the bivariate analysis of depression and fatigue.

Finally, because of the categorical nature of the data, the analyses have almost certainly lacked power. This is reflected in the wide confidence intervals in both univariate and bivariate analyses.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None. Funded by a grant from the PPP Charitable Trust.

References

Akaike, H. (1987) Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angold, A. & Costello, E. J. (2000) The Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment (CAPA). Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 3948.Google Scholar
Brace, M. J., Scott Smith, M., McCauley, E., et al (2000) Family reinforcement of illness behavior: a comparison of adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome, juvenile arthritis, and healthy controls. Journal of Developmental and Behavioural Pediatrics, 21, 332339.Google Scholar
Carter, B. D., Kronenberger, W. G., Edwards, J. F., et al (1999) Psychological symptoms in chronic fatigue and juvenile rheumatoid arthritis. Pediatrics, 103, 975979.Google Scholar
Cohen, D. J., Dibble, E., Grawe, J. M., et al (1975) Reliably separating identical from fraternal twins. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, 13711375.Google Scholar
Cooper, P. J. & Goodyer, I. (1993) A community study of depression in adolescent girls. I: estimates of symptoms and syndrome prevalence. British Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 369374.Google Scholar
Costello, E. J. & Angold, A. (1988) Scales to assess child and adolescent depression: checklists, screens, and nets. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 726737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dowsett, E. G. & Colby, J. (1997) Long term sickness absence due to ME/CFS in UK schools: an epidemiological study with medical and educational implications. Journal of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome, 3, 2942.Google Scholar
Falconer, D. S. (1965) The inheritance of liability to certain diseases, estimated from the incidence among relatives. Annals of Human Genetics, 29, 5176.Google Scholar
Farmer, A., Scourfield, J., Martin, N., et al (1999) Is disabling fatigue in childhood influenced by genes? Psychological Medicine, 29, 279282.Google Scholar
Farmer, A., Fowler, T., Scourfield, J., et al (2004) Prevalence of chronic disabling fatigue in children and adolescents. British Journal of Psychiatry, 184, 477481.Google Scholar
Fowler, T., Duthie, P., Thapar, A., et al (2005) The definition of disabling fatigue in children and adolescents. BMC Family Practice, 6, 33.Google Scholar
Fukuda, K., Straus, S. E., Hickie, I., et al (1994) The chronic fatigue syndrome: a comprehensive approach to its definition and study. International Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Study Group. Annals of Internal Medicine, 121, 953959.Google Scholar
Garralda, M. E. & Rangel, L. (2002) Annotation: chronic fatigue syndrome in children and adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 169176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodyer, I. M., Cooper, P. J., Vize, C. M., et al (1993) Depression in 11–16 year old girls: the role of past parental psychopathology and exposure to recent life events. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 11031115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatcher, S. & House, A. (2003) Life events, difficulties and dilemmas in the on set of chronic fatigue syndrome: a case–control study Psychological Medicine, 33, 11851192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickie, I., Kirk, K. & Martin, N. (1999) Unique genetic and environmental determinants of prolonged fatigue: a twin study. Psychological Medicine, 29, 259268.Google Scholar
Loehlin, R. & Nichols, R. C. (1976) Heredity, Environment and Personality. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Marshall, G. S. (1999) Report of a workshop on the epidemiology, natural history and pathogenesis of chronic fatigue syndrome in adolescents. Journal of Pediatrics, 134, 395405.Google Scholar
Meltzer, H., Gatward, R., Goodman, R., et al (2000) Mental Health of Children and Adolescents in Great Britain. London: TSO (The Stationery Office).Google Scholar
Monroe, S. M., Rohde, P., Seeley, J. R., et al (1999) Life events and depression in adolescence: relationship loss as a prospective risk factor for first onset of major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 606614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neale, M. C. (1999) MX: Statistical Modelling. Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry, Medical College of Virginia.Google Scholar
Neale, M. C. & Cardon, L. R. (1992) Methodology for Genetic Studies of Twins and Families. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Public Health Common Data Set (1993) Guildford, UK: Institute of Public Health, University of Surrey.Google Scholar
Rice, F., Harold, G. T. & Thapar, A. (2002) Assessing the effects of age, sex and shared environment on the genetic aetiology of depression in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 10391051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rijsdijk, F. V., Snieder, H., Ormel, J., et al (2003) Genetic and environmental influences on psychological distress in the population: General Health Questionnaires analyses in UK twins. Psychological Medicine, 33, 793801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Pickles, A., Murray, R., et al (2001) Testing hypotheses on specific environmental causal effects on behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 291–23.Google Scholar
Scourfield, J., Martin, N., Lewis, G., et al (1999) Heritability of social cognitive skills in children and adolescents. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 559564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scourfield, J., Rice, F., Thapar, A., et al (2003) Depressive symptoms in children and adolescents: changing aetiological influences with development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 968976.Google Scholar
Sullivan, P. F., Kovalenko, P., York, T. P., et al (2003) Fatigue in a community sample of twins. Psychological Medicine, 33, 263281.Google Scholar
Thapar, A. & McGuffin, P. (1994) A twin study of depressive symptoms in childhood. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 259265.Google Scholar
Thapar, A. & McGuffin, P. (1998) Validity of the shortened Mood and Fee ings Questionnaire in a community sample of children and adolescents: a preliminary research note. Psychiatry Research, 81, 259268.Google Scholar
Thapar, A., Harrington, R., Ross, K., et al (2000) Does the definition of ADHD affect heritability? Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 15281536.Google Scholar
Walford, G. A., Nelson, W. M. & McCluskey, D. R. (1993) Fatigue, depression, and social adjustment in chronic fatigue syndrome. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 68, 384388.Google Scholar
Williamson, R. J., Purcell, S., Sterne, A., et al (2005) The relationship of fatigue to mental and physical health in a community sample. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 40, 126132.Google Scholar
Wood, A., Kroll, L., Moore, A., et al (1995) Properties of the Mood and Feelings Questionnaire in adolescent psychiatric outpatients – A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 327334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Path diagram showing latent genetic and environmental influences on twin-rated depression and parent-rated short-duration fatigue. A, additive genetic; C, shared environment; E, non-shared environment; *significant at P=0.05. Standardised path coefficients are provided. These must be squared to obtain proportions of variance of the measured variable accounted for the latent variable.

Figure 1

Table 1 Univariate genetic model-fitting for parent-rated short-duration fatigue and prolonged fatigue using full and nested models

Figure 2

Table 2 Bivariate genetic model-fitting for twin-rated depression and parent-rated disabling fatigue using full models

Figure 3

Fig. 2 Path diagram showing latent genetic and environmental influences on twin-rated depression and parent-rated prolonged fatigue. A, additive genetic; C, shared environment; E, non-shared environment; *significant at P=0.05. Standardised path coefficients are provided. These must be squared to obtain proportions of variance of the measured variable accounted for the latent variable.

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.