Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-nwzlb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T12:13:48.675Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Genetic hypotheses for schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

T. Crow*
Affiliation:
University Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK. Email: tim.crow@psych.ox.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2007 

In their ‘common disease – rare alleles’ hypothesis McClellan et al (Reference McClellan, Susser and King2007) come close to formulating an untestable theory. Although they refer to currently fashionable ‘candidate genes’ – e.g. dysbindin, neuregulin and DISC1 – it appears that they do not regard these as established. I agree that there is no consistency in the findings across even the largest genome scans conducted to date (sample sizes exceeding 300 and totally over 1000 sib-pairs; Reference CrowCrow, 2007) but disagree profoundly about the alternative.

We know that in schizophrenia: (a) incidences are more uniform across populations as one moves to the core syndrome, e.g. nuclear symptoms (Reference Jablensky, Sartorius and ErnbergJablensky et al, 1992); (b) structural brain changes (e.g. ventricular enlargement) are consistent across populations (Reference Chua, Lam and TaiChua et al, 2003) and uniform across patients relative to controls (Reference Vita, Dieci and SilenziVita et al, 2000); (c) age at onset has a specific distribution throughout the reproductive phase of life; (d) there are gender differences (earlier onset and worse outcome in males); (e) the core syndrome comprises symptoms that are language related (i.e. specific to Homo sapiens). None of these findings would be expected if schizophrenia were a result of random mutations in a large number of genes such as McClellan et al postulate, nor would one expect variation in the form of illness within families as is generally observed.

While McClellan et al's hypothesis promises a search for elusive rare alleles that will never reach a conclusion, Craddock et al (Reference Craddock, O'Donovan and Owen2007) perseverate in their claim that ‘Several genes have been implicated repeatedly as conferring risk for schizophrenia or bipolar disorder’. Comparison of the largest and most systematic linkage studies, including those of Craddock et al themselves, shows that these claims cannot be sustained (Reference CrowCrow, 2007).

Alternative hypotheses to the ‘rare alleles of major effect’ and the ‘polygenes of small effect’ deserve consideration. One such hypothesis (Reference CrowCrow, 2007) is that the variation arises in relation to characteristics that are specifically human, i.e. recent in evolution, and that it is ‘epigenetic’ in form, i.e. involves a modification of the sequence (methylation of DNA) or the associated chromosomal structure (methylation, phosphorylation or acetylation of histones) rather than a change in the DNA sequence itself. We do know that the risk for first-degree relatives is approximately 10%, whereas that for second-degree relatives is very little increased compared with the population as a whole. Although this is often held to be consistent with polygenic influence, it is also compatible with an ‘imprint’ that is applied and reapplied in meiosis (i.e. with a short time course between generations). The solution proposed is that the variation arises in relation to the change (speciation event) that defined the species, and that this is associated with the cerebral torque – the bias from right frontal to left occipital across the anteroposterior axis that is characteristic of the human brain. In contrast to McClellan et al's rare alleles and Craddock et al's polygenes of small effect, this hypothesis is specific and refutable – a gene has been identified that duplicated at 6 million years from the X to the Y chromosome to give rise to the ProtocadherinX/Y gene pair. This pair has been subject to accelerated evolution since the duplication event (Reference Williams, Close and GiouzeliWilliams et al, 2006) and is in an unusual situation with respect to epigenetic modulation. This variation can be assessed and the hypothesis thereby tested.

Footnotes

Contents ▪ Genetic hypotheses for schizophrenia ▪ Anticipation and the genetics of psychosis ▪ Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and mood disorders in adults

References

Chua, S. E., Lam, I. W. S., Tai, K. S., et al (2003) Brain morphological abnormality in schizophrenia is independent of country of origin. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 108, 269275.Google Scholar
Craddock, N., O'Donovan, M. C. & Owen, M. J. (2007) Phenotypic and genetic complexity of psychosis. Invited commentary on … Schizophrenia: a common disease caused by multiple rare alleles. British journal of Psychiatry, 190, 200203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crow, T. J. (2007) How and why genetic linkage has not solved the problem of psychosis: review and hypothesis. American journal of Psychiatry, 164, 1321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jablensky, A., Sartorius, N., Ernberg, G., et al (1992) Schizophrenia: manifestations, incidence and course in different cultures. A World Health Organization ten-country study. Psychological Medicine Monograph Supplement, 20, 197.Google Scholar
McClellan, J. M., Susser, E. & King, M.-C. (2007) Schizophrenia: a common disease caused by multiple rare alleles. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190, 194199.Google Scholar
Vita, A., Dieci, M., Silenzi, C., et al (2000) Cerebral ventricular enlargement as a generalized feature of schizophrenia: a distribution analysis on 502 subjects. Schizophrenia Research, 44, 2534.Google Scholar
Williams, N. A., Close, J., Giouzeli, M., et al (2006) Accelerated evolution of ProcadherinllX/Y: a candidate gene-pair for cerebral asymmetry and language. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 141, 623633.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.