Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c4f8m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T09:50:03.576Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

From the Editor's desk

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2010 

Prudence for students

I once had a junior psychiatrist working for me who was very reluctant to make clinical decisions. She was very well read and when I asked her why she had not taken a course of action that most others would have thought was commonplace she defended her decision with the words ‘I thought it was imprudent to do so’, together with a long explanation which essentially could be summarised in the words I have used previously in this column, ‘If our predictions are wrong and we intervene unnecessarily, we create iatrogenic disease and much more’. Reference Tyrer1 This issue illustrates both the merits and handicaps of this philosophy. If we listen carefully to evidence and act accordingly, we may sun ourselves in the warmth of the latest guidelines but fool ourselves. Kessing et al (pp. ) and Vinogradova et al (pp. ) show both the higher risk of diabetes with antipsychotic drugs (although some may be surprised at the main offenders) and their mortality risk, yet such treatment is recommended almost universally. My passive junior colleague would never initiate antipsychotic drug treatment on account of its dangers and could point to Munafò & Flint (pp. ) in suggesting that much of their value is based on false positives, and that when schizophrenia is untreated the mortality is still the same. Reference Ran, Chan, Chen, Mao, Hu and Tang2 But the proponents of focused medical intervention, including now an Italian support axis to our UK group colleagues (Altamura & Goodwin, pp. ), Reference Craddock, Antebi, Attenburrow, Bailey, Carson and Cowen3,Reference Bullmore, Fletcher and Jones4 would point not only to clinical evidence of symptom relief, but to the disability created by psychosis, even at an early stage in the disorder (Velthorst et al, pp. ). We also need to be aware of cognitive impairment, particularly in core schizophrenic illness, Reference Bora, Yucel and Pantelis5 and the potential importance of this in classifying and treating the condition. Reference Keefe and Fenton6 The evidence that cannabis impairs memory is therefore particularly relevant to the debate over its association with schizophrenia Reference Zammit, Moore, Lingford-Hughes, Barnes, Jones and Burke7 and the apparent reversal of this effect by cannabidiol, also found in cannabis but in differing proportions, Reference Di Forti, Morgan, Dazzan, Pariante, Mondelli and Marques8 reported by Morgan et al (pp. ) offers the enticing possibility that cannabidiol may offer an acceptable alternative to other antipsychotic drugs (Henquet & Kuepper, pp. ) that is not associated with increased mortality.

So if we are to advance our knowledge and therapeutic wisdom, we have to take some chances and throughout this process we have to have a judicious balance between benefit and risk. I would be surprised if anyone disagreed with this; the problem is deciding how ‘judicious’ is defined. I have to say I found my younger colleague's approach infuriating because it seemed such a negative view of our craft – the Hippocratic dictum of primum non nocere was perfectly appropriate in ancient Greece when most medical interventions had no demonstrable benefit, but we now have much more of value from which to choose. I remain an enthusiast and optimist with regard to new treatments. ‘Prudence keeps life safe, but does not often make it happy’, said Samuel Johnson, and we should be prepared to offer the chance to give our patients joy – cannabis excepted.

Service users centre stage

My views about service users – I still prefer the term ‘patients' as it is more accurate and universal in medicine – have changed somewhat over recent years. Not so long ago I felt that the growth of the user movement might handicap research in mental health because consumerism might take over from evidence, ‘accelerate out of control and drive mental health research into the sand’. Reference Tyrer9 It has not done so, and I am pleased to admit I was wrong. I think the reason for this is that service user opinions have become much more representative and less individual-focused. An ‘expert by experience’ is a limited expert if he or she has only direct personal experience of care to report. But ‘collections of experience’, obtained systematically from a wide range of service users, comprise very useful data and are illustrated well by Singh et al (pp. ) in their study of the general inadequacies of the transition from child to adult mental health services. When all the data, both qualitative and quantitative, point in the same direction then we have a powerful combination indeed. What a pity ethical considerations prevented service users outside the system from contributing to this valuable database. We have similar powerful evidence from other studies, particularly in the management of self-harm where attitudes have changed enormously in the past few years, largely because service users have spoken out and their voices are being heard. Reference Taylor, Hawton, Fortune and Kapur10 Collaboration is the essential word in this context and, although one of my non-English colleagues says he detests the word as in his country all collaborators are shot, we need to reclaim it for the team effort that makes lasting progress.

References

1 Tyrer, P. From the Editor's Desk. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Ran, MS, Chan, CL-W, Chen, EY-H, Mao, W-J, Hu, S-H, Tang, C-P, et al. Differences in mortality and suicidal behaviour between treated and never-treated people with schizophrenia in rural China. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 126–31.Google Scholar
3 Craddock, N, Antebi, D, Attenburrow, M-J, Bailey, A, Carson, A, Cowen, P, et al. Wake-up call for British psychiatry. Br J Psychiatry 2008; 193: 69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Bullmore, E, Fletcher, P, Jones, PB. Why psychiatry can't afford to be neurophobic. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 194: 293–5.Google Scholar
5 Bora, E, Yucel, M, Pantelis, C. Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder and affective psychoses: meta-analytic study. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 475–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 Keefe, RSE, Fenton, WS. How should DSM–V criteria for schizophrenia include cognitive impairment? Schizophr Bull 2007; 33: 912–20.Google Scholar
7 Zammit, S, Moore, THM, Lingford-Hughes, A, Barnes, TRE, Jones, PB, Burke, M, et al. Effects of cannabis use on outcomes of psychotic disorders: systematic review. Br J Psychiatry 2008; 193: 357–63.Google Scholar
8 Di Forti, M, Morgan, C, Dazzan, P, Pariante, C, Mondelli, V, Marques, TR, et al. High-potency cannabis and the risk of psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 488–91.Google Scholar
9 Tyrer, P. Commentary: research into health services needs a new approach. Psychiatr Bull 2002; 26: 406–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
10 Taylor, TL, Hawton, K, Fortune, S, Kapur, N. Attitudes towards clinical services among people who self-harm: systematic review. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 194: 104–10.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.