Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T09:40:24.643Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Authors' reply

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Hossein Hassanian-Moghaddam
Affiliation:
Department of Internal Medicine, Division of Clinical Toxicology, Faculty of Medicine, Shahid Beheshti University MC, Tehran, Iran. Email: hassanian@sbmu.ac.ir
Gregory Carter
Affiliation:
Centre for Brain and Mental Health Research, Faculty of Health, University of Newcastle, Australia
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2011 

Drs Jhangee & Bhatia have mentioned a number of strengths and limitations, which were specifically addressed in the paper. The other issues that were raised are addressed below.

  1. (a) Postcards are a minimal intervention sustained over 12 months. Optimal assessment is end of treatment and at follow-up, which allows comparison with similar studies. Reference Carter, Clover, Whyte, Dawson and D'Este1,Reference Carter, Clover, Whyte, Dawson and D'Este2 Repeated contact and assessment might ‘wash out’ the effect of intervention and telephone contacts might specifically influence suicide attempts. Reference Vaiva, Ducrocq, Meyer, Mathieu, Philippe and Libersa3 The costs for three assessments for over 2000 participants would have been considerable and the additional benefits of end-points measured before treatment completion are unlikely to offset the additional costs.

  2. (b) Instruments assessing suicidal intention (rather than ideation) are contextualised to an episode of self-harm, suicide attempt or ideation. These were relatively uncommon and so intention would only have been measureable in a minority, if there was an instrument for the relevant languages and shown to be valid in the study population. Had there been such an instrument it might have been considered for baseline assessment.

  3. (c) Using consecutive admissions is superior to any alternate sampling strategy. We acknowledged the limitations of restriction to a 4-month period.

  4. (d) Psychiatric diagnostic assessments were done for all in-patients. We were mindful of the dangers of subgroup analyses in general. Initially we analysed for gender based on benefit only for women Reference Carter, Clover, Whyte, Dawson and D'Este1,Reference Carter, Clover, Whyte, Dawson and D'Este2 and a differential gender repetition rate of self-harm or poisoning in Western populations. We accepted the editorial suggestion of a second analysis based on previous suicide attempt at baseline, since this might be the highest risk factor for subsequent suicidal behaviour. Postcards in Persia and Postcards from the EDge intended to develop interventions available to almost all emergency departments with patients who had self-harmed, even emergency departments without psychiatric services required for diagnosis; so analysis based on psychiatric diagnosis was of low importance. We have tested alternate approaches to psychiatric diagnosis, which had low agreement with clinical diagnosis. Reference Jayasekera, Carter and Clover4

  5. (e) There were several post hoc analyses based on recall of the number of postcards received. Since this was an efficacy trial, we conducted the main analyses based on randomisation, not exposure or dosage of the intervention.

  6. (f) The research psychologist was not masked to allocation and may have inadvertently influenced responses at follow-up. Participants may have guessed the study end-points from questions asked of them, but their reports of the hospital-treated suicide attempts were found to be accurate.

  7. (g) There were two points in the paper that suggested that a substantial response bias was unlikely. The report of hospital treated episodes was accurate. Although ideation and attempt were significantly different, self-cutting was not, which would require a differential response bias in favour of two outcomes but against another.

  8. (h) It would be useful to know the reasons for withdrawal. However, less than 2.3% of the treatment group withdrew, suggesting acceptability was rather good and improved retention in treatment would be small. The most innovative analysis addressed the issue of the possible impact caused by individuals withdrawn or lost to follow-up. We expect that sensitivity analyses Reference Hollis5 that account for all possible outcomes might become a future standard for reporting randomised controlled trials that cannot guarantee an intention-to-treat analysis based on all participants or which rely on imputed values for non-ignorable missing binary end points.

References

1 Carter, GL, Clover, K, Whyte, IM, Dawson, AH, D'Este, C. Postcards from the EDge project: randomised controlled trial of an intervention using postcards to reduce repetition of hospital treated deliberate self poisoning. BMJ 2005; 331: 805–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Carter, GL, Clover, K, Whyte, IM, Dawson, AH, D'Este, C. Postcards from the EDge: 24-month outcomes of a randomised controlled trial for hospital-treated self-poisoning. Br J Psychiatry 2007; 191: 548–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3 Vaiva, G, Ducrocq, F, Meyer, P, Mathieu, D, Philippe, A, Libersa, C, et al. Effect of telephone contact on further suicide attempts in patients discharged from an emergency department: randomised controlled study. BMJ 2006; 332: 1241–5.Google Scholar
4 Jayasekera, H, Carter, G, Clover, K. Comparison of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI-Auto) with clinical diagnosis in a suicidal population. Arch Suicide Res 2011; 15: 4355.Google Scholar
5 Hollis, S. A graphical sensitivity analysis for clinical trials with non-ignorable missing binary outcome. Stat Med 2002; 21: 3823–34.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.