Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T18:07:14.444Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

From the Editor's desk

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Columns
Copyright
Copyright © 2012 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Humdrum conundrums

Talking to a group of general practitioners recently, after presenting what I thought were some pretty impressive data showing why we get so exercised about good diagnosis, I was asked, ‘why do you bother with diagnosis at all in psychiatry when you don't know the answers?’ My questioner would doubtless find this issue of the Journal boring and anodyne, as many papers are concerned with the complex interrelationship of four common disorders – between anxiety and depression and between psychopathy and antisocial behaviour. One of the reasons why these topics receive so much attention is that they account for a large part of the total psychopathology that exists in the community, probably as much as 20%, Reference Jenkins, Lewis, Bebbington, Brugha, Farrell and Gill1,Reference Coid, Yang, Tyrer, Roberts and Ullrich2 maybe more when subthreshold conditions are taken into account. Reference Walters, Buszewicz, Weich and King3

The purpose of diagnosis in psychiatry is to aid communication and understanding, promote correct treatment and predict outcome. I have not added ‘identify pathology’ because although this is also true we are still far from certain where normality ends and pathology begins with the common mental disorders. Reference Maj4 The findings of Coryell et al (pp. ) and others, Reference Andreescu, Lenze, Dew, Begley, Mulsant and Dombrovski5Reference Klein Hofmeijer–Sevink, Batelaan, van Megen, Penninx, Cath and van den Hout7 including Bogic et al (pp. ), confirm, if indeed such confirmation were needed, that whatever differences there are between anxiety and depression as symptoms they are close brothers in psychopathology and, like close brothers, stay together to the end, and do not assist remission in any form. Our primary care colleagues regard the anxiety–depression conundrum as irritating and tedious; they cannot understand why we cannot sort it out rather better than we have done to date, and the acronym covering mixed anxiety and depressive disorder is an apposite index of their frustration. Biological explanations, as Andreescu & Lenze (pp. ) acknowledge, always seem to be at the point of providing an answer but, as with Newton in his later years, never reach the point of converting these base diagnoses into gold. But we have to continue the quest as it is so important clinically. In this issue, for example, Huntley et al (pp. ) suggest the potential value of group treatment for depression, but their review does not include anxiety factors at all, even though these are likely to be highly relevant when considering the merits of, and personal choices for, individual and group treatment.

Psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder are even closer brothers, often in crime. Our diagnostic systems have never got to grips with psychopathy as a diagnostic entity and so it does not appear in our classification systems except as antisocial personality disorder or, latterly, as dangerous and severe personality disorder (Sinclair et al, pp. ). It is clear that personality disorder arises in childhood no matter when it is diagnosed and the work of Kumsta et al (pp. ) points to a genuine distinction between conduct disorder and callous–unemotional disorder, or psychopathy, at a young age. This is reinforced by Dadds et al (pp. ) and by Viding & McCrory (pp. ) in their lively contributions, and there is some biological support for this diagnostic separation. Reference Raine, Lee, Yang and Colletti8 Rutter (pp. ) makes a strong case for serious consideration of conduct disorder and psychopathy as separate conditions but recognises the need for caution in formal classification. The common outcome of such diagnostic debates is ‘further testing needed’ and the uncertainty over the place of conditions such as ADHD Reference Langley, Fowler, Ford, Thapar, van den Bree and Harold9 and the limited number of longer-term outcome studies Reference Fergusson, Boden, Horwood, Miller and Kennedy10 reinforces this caution. In the ICD-11 Reference Skodol11 and DSM-5 Reference Tyrer, Crawford, Mulder, Blashfield, Farnam and Fossati12 proposed revisions of the classification of personality disorder, psychopathy as an entity is unlikely to appear. My own feeling, possibly a prejudice, is that intelligent, charming and manipulative antisocial people, highly intriguing to the media, Reference Byrne13 are called psychopaths and that unintelligent, recidivist, charmless vagabonds are dismissed as antisocial. But enquiries will continue and I can reply to my GP friend cheerily: ‘of course we don't know the answers, sir, but isn't the search for solutions more exciting than finding what is already known?’

Our typical reader

Those of you who are unfortunate enough to receive a rejection letter for an article submitted to the Journal will find it often includes a sentence suggesting the paper is not in the ‘mainstream of interest for our readers, who are mainly clinical psychiatrists’. Here I am taking a leap into the unknown as I have no right to arrogate to myself the collective wisdom of our readership, but I thought it worthwhile to at least state what I think our readers would like from our Journal, and then wait for corrections, which I am sure will be many. First, there are many who read the Journal who are not psychiatrists, and we need to take account of their interests too. Indeed, we embrace all who are interested in mental health and our former title, Journal of Mental Science, indicated we cover a broad sweep. Next, I perceive that most of our readers are not primarily academic, but interested in getting on with understanding and treating those who are ill, not in faffing around at the edges. Finally, they want a bit of light relief from time to time, and I hope they can find it in the pages of the Journal. In short, our typical reader is someone who:

‘Reads a little, but thinks a lot

Needs a modicum of cheer

Takes breakthroughs with a pinch of salt

Yearns a simple path to steer’

References

1 Jenkins, R, Lewis, G, Bebbington, P, Brugha, T, Farrell, M, Gill, B, et al. The National Psychiatric Morbidity surveys of Great Britain: initial findings from the household survey. Psychol Med 1997; 27: 775–89.Google Scholar
2 Coid, J, Yang, M, Tyrer, P, Roberts, A, Ullrich, S. Prevalence and correlates of personality disorder in Great Britain. Br J Psychiatry 2006; 188: 423–31Google Scholar
3 Walters, K, Buszewicz, M, Weich, S, King, M. Mixed anxiety and depressive disorder outcomes: prospective cohort study in primary care. Br J Psychiatry 2011; 198: 472–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Maj, M. When does depression become a mental disorder? Br J Psychiatry 2011; 199: 85–6.Google Scholar
5 Andreescu, C, Lenze, EJ, Dew, MA, Begley, AE, Mulsant, BH, Dombrovski, AY, et al. Effect of comorbid anxiety on treatment response and relapse risk in late–life depression: controlled study. Br J Psychiatry 2007; 190: 344–9.Google Scholar
6 Das–Munshi, J, Goldberg, D, Bebbington, PE, Bhugra, DK, Brugha, TS, Dewey, ME, et al. Public health significance of mixed anxiety and depression: beyond current classification. Br J Psychiatry 2008; 192: 171–7.Google Scholar
7 Klein Hofmeijer–Sevink, M, Batelaan, NM, van Megen, HJ, Penninx, BW, Cath, DC, van den Hout, MA, et al. Clinical relevance of comorbidity in anxiety disorders: a report from the Netherlands Study of Depression and Anxiety (NESDA). J Affect Disord 2012; Jan 10, epub ahead of print, doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.12.008.Google Scholar
8 Raine, A, Lee, L, Yang, Y, Colletti, P. Neurodevelopmental marker for limbic maldevelopment in antisocial personality disorder and psychopathy. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 197: 186–92.Google Scholar
9 Langley, K, Fowler, T, Ford, T, Thapar, AK, van den Bree, M, Harold, G, et al. Adolescent clinical outcomes for young people with attention–deficit hyperactivity disorder. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 196: 235–40.Google Scholar
10 Fergusson, DM, Boden, JM, Horwood, LJ, Miller, AL, Kennedy, MA. MAOA, abuse exposure and antisocial behaviour: 30–year longitudinal study. Br J Psychiatry 2011; 198: 457–63.Google Scholar
11 Skodol, AE. Scientific issues in the revision of personality disorders for DSM–5. Personality and Mental Health 2011; 5: 97111.Google Scholar
12 Tyrer, P, Crawford, M, Mulder, R, Blashfield, R, Farnam, A, Fossati, A, et al. The rationale for the reclassification of personality disorder in the 11th revision of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD–11). Personality and Mental Health 2011; 5: 246–59.Google Scholar
13 Byrne, P. And the winner is ⃛ the loser – psychiatry in the movies. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 197: 304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.