Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-p566r Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T08:25:14.835Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impact of cognitive therapy on internalised stigma in people with at-risk mental states

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Anthony P. Morrison*
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester and Greater Manchester West Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester
Max Birchwood
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham
Melissa Pyle
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester and Greater Manchester West Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester
Clare Flach
Affiliation:
Centre for Biostatistics, Institute of Population Health, University of Manchester, Manchester
Suzanne L. K. Stewart
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester, Manchester
Rory Byrne
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester and Greater Manchester West Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester
Paul Patterson
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham
Peter B. Jones
Affiliation:
School of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge
David Fowler
Affiliation:
School of Medicine, Health Policy, and Practice, University of East Anglia, Norwich
Andrew I. Gumley
Affiliation:
School of Medicine, Health Policy, and Practice, University of East Anglia, Norwich
Paul French
Affiliation:
Greater Manchester West Mental Health NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester, UK
*
Anthony P. Morrison, School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester, Oxford Road, Manchester M13 9PL, UK. Email: tony.morrison@manchester.ac.uk.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Background

Internalised stigma in young people meeting criteria for at-risk mental states (ARMS) has been highlighted as an important issue, and it has been suggested that provision of cognitive therapy may increase such stigma.

Aims

To investigate the effects of cognitive therapy on internalised stigma using a secondary analysis of data from the EDIE-2 trial.

Method

Participants meeting criteria for ARMS were recruited as part of a multisite randomised controlled trial of cognitive therapy for prevention and amelioration of psychosis. Participants were assessed at baseline and at 6, 12, 18 and 24 months using measures of psychotic experiences, symptoms and internalised stigma.

Results

Negative appraisals of experiences were significantly reduced in the group assigned to cognitive therapy (estimated difference at 12 months was −1.36 (95% Cl −2.69 to −0.02), P = 0.047). There was no difference in social acceptability of experiences (estimated difference at 12 months was 0.46, 95% Cl −0.05 to 0.98, P = 0.079).

Conclusions

These findings suggest that, rather than increasing internalised stigma, cognitive therapy decreases negative appraisals of unusual experiences in young people at risk of psychosis; as such, it is a non-stigmatising intervention for this population.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2013 

Reliable and valid criteria are available to identify help-seeking individuals who are at increased risk of developing schizophrenia and related psychoses. Yung and colleagues Reference Yung, McGorry, McFarlane, Jackson, Patton and Rakkar1 developed such criteria to identify three subgroups experiencing an at-risk mental state (ARMS) for psychosis in individuals aged 14-35 years: two subgroups with state risk factors, defined by the presence of either transient psychotic symptoms or attenuated (subclinical) psychotic symptoms; the other subgroup with trait-plus-state risk factors. These criteria have now been widely adopted internationally, and there have been recent debates about the utility and potential stigma Reference Yang, Wonpat-Borja, Opler and Corcoran2 of including an attenuated psychosis syndrome in DSM-5. Reference Corcoran, First and Cornblatt3,Reference Morrison, Byrne and Bentall4 Several randomised controlled trials (RCTs) have evaluated the impact of psychosocial and pharmacological interventions Reference Addington, Epstein, Liu, French, Boydell and Zipursky5-Reference Morrison, French, Stewart, Birchwood, Fowler and Gumley12 on transition to psychosis and severity of psychotic symptoms within this population, and a recent meta-analysis concluded that cognitive therapy may offer a particularly encouraging approach to prevention of psychosis. Reference Stafford, Jackson, Mayo-Wilson, Morrison and Kendall13 However, there is debate about the ethics and unintended consequences of identifying people as being at risk of psychosis and providing any treatment to such a population. Reference Warner14 Whereas there are clear adverse effects associated with pharmacological interventions such as antipsychotics, Reference Corcoran, Malaspina and Hercher15 the risk of iatrogenic harm may be lower with psychosocial interventions. The Early Detection and Intervention Evaluation 2 (EDIE-2) trial recently demonstrated that cognitive therapy seems to be an acceptable treatment, with a high adherence rate. It found that although cognitive therapy did not significantly reduce transition to psychosis or symptom-related distress (in the context of a low overall transition rate), it did reduce severity of psychotic symptoms in people at high risk. Reference Morrison, French, Stewart, Birchwood, Fowler and Gumley12 Another notable finding was that the majority of participants in both groups improved over time. However, it has been suggested that psychological treatments such as cognitive therapy may unintentionally increase internalised stigma within this population. Reference Yang, Wonpat-Borja, Opler and Corcoran2,Reference Warner14 The opposite has also been argued in relation to cognitive therapy, with claims that the use of a normalising approach to understanding psychotic experiences should reduce internalised stigma. Reference French and Morrison16,Reference Bentall and Morrison17 Internalised stigma has been defined as ‘becoming aware of the label and identifying with the stereotypes’ Reference Link, Yang, Phelan and Collins18 or ‘the internalisation of shame, blame, hopelessness, guilt and fear of discrimination associated with mental illness’, Reference Corrigan and Watson19 and encompasses the idea that those with mental health problems experience both shame of their diagnosis and fear of discrimination. Reference Ritsher, Otilingam and Grajales20 Given that this area is unexplored in relation to people identified as being at risk of psychosis, our current study uses a secondary analysis of data from the EDIE-2 trial Reference Morrison, French, Stewart, Birchwood, Fowler and Gumley12 in order to test the hypothesis that in comparison to a symptom-monitoring control, cognitive therapy will reduce internalised stigma in young people with an ARMS.

Method

Trial design

The EDIE-2 trial is a multisite randomised, controlled, single-blind (rater) study comparing two conditions (cognitive therapy plus mental state monitoring v. a mental state monitoring control). The randomisation algorithm used randomised permuted blocks with block sizes of six or eight, after first stratifying by site and gender. Assessors were masked to treatment condition and many diverse strategies were employed to achieve masked ratings. More details of the trial methodology, including a CONSORT flow diagram, are available in the primary outcome report. Reference Morrison, French, Stewart, Birchwood, Fowler and Gumley12

Participants

Entry criteria for the EDIE-2 trial Reference Morrison, French, Stewart, Birchwood, Fowler and Gumley12 were assessed using the Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States (CAARMS), which is specifically designed for the assessment of ARMS. Reference Yung, Yuen, McGorry, Phillips, Kelly and Dell'Olio21 The entry routes consist of individuals experiencing brief limited intermittent psychotic symptoms, attenuated psychotic symptoms or state-plus-trait factors. Other inclusion criteria were aged 14-35 and help-seeking. Five sites were involved: Manchester, Birmingham/Worcestershire, Glasgow & Clyde, Cambridgeshire and Norfolk. Participants were predominantly identified by health professionals working within diverse agencies within primary- and secondary-care settings; our most common sources were early intervention services (34%), primary care (GPs/primary care mental health teams: 21%) and youth/student counselling services (15%). We recruited 288 participants; 144 were allocated to cognitive therapy and 144 to monitoring only.

Measures

Eligibility for the study was measured using the CAARMS, Reference Yung, Yuen, McGorry, Phillips, Kelly and Dell'Olio21 which provided measures of transition to psychosis, symptom severity and associated distress. The interrater reliability of the CAARMS assessments was assessed regularly over the lifetime of the trial and intraclass correlation coefficients indicated good interrater reliability (mean 0.90, s.d. = 0.03).

Internalised stigma was measured using the Personal Beliefs about Experiences Questionnaire (PBEQ; details available from the authors on request), which is a 13-item self-report questionnaire; each item reflects social and cultural beliefs/stereotypes about psychosis (for example ‘I can talk to most people about my experiences’). Participants rate the degree to which they endorse statements to be true about themselves on a four-point scale (one to four): ‘strongly disagree’, ‘disagree’, ‘agree’ and ‘strongly agree’. The PBEQ is an adapted version of the Personal Beliefs about Illness Questionnaire (PBIQ); Reference Birchwood, Mason, MacMillan and Healy22,Reference Birchwood, Jackson, Brunet, Holden and Barton23 minor revisions were made in order to make the questionnaire suitable for use with people meeting criteria for ARMS. These revisions involved replacing the word ‘illness’ with ‘experience’ and the removal of three items that were not applicable to the ARMS population. Exploratory factor analysis of the PBEQ with the ARMS population has suggested two subscales for this measure: negative appraisals of unusual experiences and the perceived social acceptability of unusual experiences. Analyses indicated good reliability for the negative appraisals subscale (α = 0.74) and acceptable reliability for the social acceptability subscale (α = 0.52), which is comparable to the reliability of the original subscales of the PBIQ. Items loading highly on the negative appraisals factor included: ‘There is something strange about me which is responsible for these experiences’, ‘It is hard for me to work or keep a job because of my experiences’ and ‘There must always have been something wrong with me as a person (to have caused these experiences)’. Items loading highly on the social acceptability factor included: ‘I can talk to most people about my experiences’, ‘My experiences mean that I should be kept away from others’ and ‘I am embarrassed to talk about my experiences’ (the latter two items being reverse scored). High scores on negative appraisals indicate higher internalised stigma, whereas lower scores on social acceptability indicate higher internalised stigma.

All participants received assessments that included both CAARMS and PBEQ at baseline, 6 months (end of treatment) and 12 months. Our variable follow-up period meant that participants recruited in the first 14 months of the study were planned to receive 24 months of follow-up; participants recruited after that were offered steadily reducing follow-up periods, depending on time of recruitment, with a minimum follow-up period of 12 months.

Procedure

Participants were randomised to one of two conditions: monitoring control or cognitive therapy plus monitoring. All participants received treatment as usual plus regular monitoring (incorporating a CAARMS assessment from a research assistant), which represents an enhancement over routine care since it aimed to provide warm, empathic and non-judgemental face-to-face contact, supportive listening, signposting to appropriate local services for unmet needs and crisis management when required. In addition to this monitoring component, participants allocated to the therapy arm of the trial received cognitive therapy based on a specific cognitive model. Reference Morrison24 Sessions were offered on a weekly basis for up to 26 weeks, plus up to four booster sessions in the subsequent 6 months. Cognitive therapy allows an individualised, problem-oriented approach and incorporates a process of assessment and formulation, which is manualised. The specific interventions depend on individual goals and formulations, but the range of permissible interventions is described in our published manual. Reference French and Morrison16 Key ingredients of the approach are the development of a problem and goal list, early formulation (both longitudinal and maintenance), a focus on normalising psychotic-like experiences and an active therapy stance utilising behavioural experiments and evaluation of appraisals. The emphasis on provision of normalising information regarding unusual experiences and the evaluation of catastrophic and shame-related or pessimistic appraisals of such experiences is particularly relevant to this study. Further details regarding the trial design and primary trial outcomes can be found elsewhere. Reference Morrison, French, Stewart, Birchwood, Fowler and Gumley12

Statistical methods

Analyses were undertaken in Stata (version 12) for Windows. The analysis strategy of the primary trial outcome was replicated for this investigation of changes in internalised stigma; primary analysis was by intention-to-treat. Repeated measures models with random effects were used with the summed PBEQ subscales as dependent variables, allowing for attrition and the variable follow-up times introduced by the design of the trial. This approach accounts for non-independence of measures within the same person. The use of these models allows for the analysis of all available data, on the assumption that data are missing at random, Reference Little and Rubin25 conditional on adjustment for centre and observed baseline scores. The models allowed for linear, quadratic and cubic trends in stigma scores over time, but only involved testing the treatment by linear trend interaction (i.e. based on the assumption that quadratic and cubic trends would be the same for both groups). The estimated parameters include a main effect of treatment, a linear and quadratic effect of follow-up time (months) and a linear effect of treatment × month interaction. Months of follow-up were centred on 12 months so that the main effect of treatment corresponds to the difference between the two arms determined at 12 months. We adjust for the stigma subscale scores at baseline and the site of the participant. We report robust standard errors, significance levels and confidence intervals.

In order to examine the effect of attendance at cognitive therapy, instrumental variable regression using the adjusted treatment-received algorithm was used in a two-stage least squares analysis on the 12-month outcomes. A regression model was first fitted for the effects of site and randomised group on sessions. Predicted values and residuals from this model were saved and used in a second regression model for the effects of sessions on outcome using the predicted values, allowing additionally for site and the baseline value of the stigma score. This two-stage procedure allows for missing outcomes assuming that they are missing at random. Reference Maracy and Dunn26

Results

The baseline characteristics of the whole sample, and the two groups, are presented in Table 1. More detailed baseline characteristics, demographics and referral pathways for the participants are described elsewhere. Reference Morrison, Stewart, French, Bentall, Birchwood and Byrne27 The participants allocated to cognitive therapy received a mean of 9.11 sessions (s.d. = 6.69, range 0-26), each session lasting on average 1 h. Adherence to cognitive therapy was reasonably good, with only 9/144 (6.25%) not attending any sessions, and 108/144 (75%) receiving at least four or more sessions. Table 2 shows the results of the PBEQ subscales (negative appraisals and social acceptability) at the 6-, 12-, 18- and 24-month end-points.

The random-effects regression analysis estimates that negative appraisal scores are 1.36 points lower in the cognitive therapy group at 12 months compared with the monitoring group (95% CI −2.69 to −0.02, P = 0.047). There is an overall reduction in negative appraisals over time (coefficient, −0.18, 95% CI −0.32 to −0.04, P = 0.012), but no significant interaction by treatment arm (coefficient, 0.05, 95% CI −0.07 to 0.17, P = 0.402). Thus, a beneficial effect of cognitive therapy is observable at 12 months, but the scores of both groups reduced significantly over the whole duration of the trial.

There was no statistically significant difference in social acceptability scores between the cognitive therapy and control arms (coefficient, 0.46, 95% CI −0.05 to 0.98, P = 0.079). Social acceptability shows a small increase over time, although this is not quite statistically significant (coefficient, 0.06, 95% CI 0.00 to 0.12, P = 0.051). There was no interaction between time and the treatment group, indicating that changes in social acceptability over time do not differ between the groups (coefficient, 0.0001, 95% CI −0.06 to 0.06, P = 0.997).

With regard to the effects of number of sessions, no significant exposure effect was observed at the 12-month outcome for either negative appraisals (sessions effect, −0.06, 95% CI −0.26 to 0.13, P = 0.521) or social acceptability (sessions effect, 0.06, 95% CI −0.02 to 0.14, P= 0.132). Thus, there was no dose-response effect.

Discussion

Main findings

This is the first study to examine the effects of any treatment on internalised stigma in people with ARMS. Our study has shown that cognitive therapy for people meeting ARMS criteria does not increase, but probably reduces, their negative appraisals of unusual experiences over 12-24 months. Cognitive therapy does not significantly improve their appraisals of the social acceptability of these experiences; however, there was a strong trend in that direction (P = 0.07), which is clearly incompatible with suggestions that it may worsen concerns about the social acceptability of psychotic experiences. These results suggest that concerns that the provision of cognitive therapy to people at risk of psychosis may be contraindicated or harmful on the basis of increasing internalised stigma Reference Warner14 are unfounded. Indeed, it would seem that, rather than increasing stigma as an adverse effect associated with treatment, cognitive therapy tends to reduce internalised stigma as has been suggested elsewhere. Reference French and Morrison16,Reference Bentall and Morrison17

Table 1 Baseline characteristics

Whole sample
(n = 288)
Cognitive therapy plus
monitoring group
(n = 144)
Monitoring only group
(n = 144)
Age, years: mean (s.d.) 20.74 (4.34) 20.73 (4.18) 20.75 (4.50)
Male: female ratio 180:108 89:55 91:53
Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States, mean (s.d.)
    Severity, summed 43.06 (18.87) 43.50 (17.65) 42.61 (20.07)
    Distress, summed 42.61 (20.03) 42.77 (20.51) 42.45 (19.62)
Personal Beliefs about Experiences Questionnaire, mean (s.d.)
    Negative appraisals 23.95 (4.25) 24.46 (4.06) 23.41 (4.39)
    Social acceptability 9.22 (2.01) 8.85 (2.01) 9.59 (1.99)

This finding has implications for the likely cost-benefit ratios associated with providing specific treatments for this group, which has been identified as of crucial importance in the ethics of intervening with an at-risk population. Reference Carpenter28 It is, therefore, important that future studies evaluating treatments for ARMS should measure internalised stigma to inform the consideration of such risk-benefit ratios, as they are likely to differ significantly between treatment approaches. It has been argued that a key mechanism of action underpinning the effectiveness of cognitive therapy is the reduction of potentially harmful appraisals of psychotic experiences via the development of normalising, decatastrophising and deshaming understanding of these experiences. Reference French and Morrison16 Our findings are compatible with such an assumption.

The non-significant increase in perceived social acceptability of experiences may reflect the possibility that changing appraisals of social acceptability is a greater challenge, since such appraisals are likely to be deep-rooted in the wider cultural context, rather than internally generated. In fact, it may be that a certain level of caution about disclosure of psychotic experiences in the current cultural environment may be adaptive, given the extent of prejudice and discrimination. Reference Thornicroft, Brohan, Rose, Sartorius and Leese29 Goffman originally described stigma as ‘an attribute which is deeply discrediting’ and as ‘an undesired differentness’, and described internalised stigma as identification with a negative stereotype. Reference Goffman30 Psychosis is one of the most stigmatised mental health problems Reference Thornicroft, Brohan, Rose, Sartorius and Leese29,Reference Brohan, Elgie, Sartorius and Thornicroft31 and stigma associated with psychosis can discourage people from seeking help. Reference Thornicroft32 This may delay treatment and leads to social isolation and reduced employment and education opportunities. Reference Thornicroft, Brohan, Rose, Sartorius and Leese29 Such stigma also results in poorer physical healthcare, suicidality and higher mortality rates. Reference Thornicroft, Rose and Kassam33 People with psychosis report internalising stigmatising social stereotypes and experience shame and fear as a consequence. Reference Corrigan, Watson and Barr34,Reference Birchwood, Trower, Brunet, Gilbert, Iqbal and Jackson35 There is a strong negative relationship between internalised stigma and a range of psychosocial variables including hope, self-esteem, empowerment and adherence with treatment; Reference Livingston and Boyd36 similar associations have recently been demonstrated in the ARMS population. Therefore, a reduction in negative appraisals of unusual experiences and a trend towards increasing the perceived social acceptability of such experiences is an important finding.

Limitations

Our trial is the largest trial with an ARMS population to date, and the use of five sites should ensure the generalisability of our findings to routine clinical service provision. However, there are some potential difficulties with this study. Our measurement of internalised stigma is potentially problematic, since the PBEQ utilised ‘my experiences’ rather than a label such as ‘illness’ (as in the original questionnaire), since this seemed more appropriate for an at-risk group yet to establish a diagnosable disorder. However, measuring internalised stigma without reference to a label or diagnosis may be difficult. Nevertheless, participants in the trial were aware that their psychotic-like symptoms were believed to increase their risk of psychosis and cognitive therapy was intended to reduce this and the component symptoms. In this context, we believe the present study provided a strong test of the stigma hypothesis. We should also point out that the social acceptability subscale had only moderate internal reliability, with an alpha of 0.52; however, the original PBIQ subscales had comparable reliability coefficients and it was the negative appraisals subscale that demonstrated a significant beneficial effect of cognitive therapy.

Table 2 Means and standard deviations for the negative appraisals and social acceptability subscales of the Personal Beliefs about Experiences Questionnaire

6 months 12 months 18 months 24 months
Cognitive therapy
groupFootnote a
Monitoring only
group
Cognitive therapy
groupFootnote a
Monitoring only
group
Cognitive therapy
groupFootnote a
Monitoring only
group
Cognitive therapy
groupFootnote a
Monitoring only
group
Variables n Mean (s.d.) n Mean (s.d.) n Mean (s.d.) n Mean (s.d.) n Mean (s.d.) n Mean (s.d.) n Mean (s.d.) n Mean (s.d.)
Negative appraisals 81 20.43 (5.23) 83 21.12 (4.96) 86 20.07 (5.89) 81 19.78 (5.04) 35 18.77 (5.89) 39 19.90 (4.66) 30 17.77 (5.46) 27 20.03 (6.34)
Social acceptability 82 10.24 (2.06) 83 10.06 (2.04) 87 10.61 (2.13) 82 10.49 (2.38) 35 11.00 (2.49) 40 10.68 (2.62) 30 11.13 (2.49) 27 10.19 (2.62)

a. Cognitive therapy group: cognitive therapy plus monitoring.

b. Numbers of participants available for follow-up at 18 and 24 months were lower than previous assessment points because we had a decreasing follow-up period for participants recruited later in the trial.

There was a significant proportion of missing data, which can introduce the possibility of bias, however, the proportions were similar for both groups, and much of the missing data were planned due to our variable length follow-up. It would have been interesting to examine the effects of transition to psychosis on internalised stigma but the rarity of this event within our sample prohibits this (only 8% developed psychosis over the lifetime of the trial). A final consideration is the clinical significance of the demonstrated improvement; it could be argued that an average reduction of 1.36 points on the negative appraisals subscale (roughly corresponding to a change from agree to disagree on one negative stereotype) is not clinically significant. However, our results clearly demonstrate that provision of cognitive therapy does not increase negative appraisals of attenuated psychotic experiences or reduce their perceived social acceptability (i.e. with respect to internalised stigma, cognitive therapy is a benign intervention).

Implications

We have demonstrated a statistically significant reduction in internalised stigma as a result of cognitive therapy. However, it remains to be seen whether the risks of external stigma and discrimination (such as that experienced from employers, families, health professionals and the general public), which are significant concerns for people with a diagnosis of schizophrenia, Reference Goffman30 may be associated with a label such as ARMS or the attenuated psychosis syndrome proposed in DSM-5. Reference Corcoran, First and Cornblatt3 Similarly, as we did not have a group who met criteria but were not informed of this fact, we cannot be sure that the influence of being assessed for risk of psychosis does not, in itself, increase internalised stigma. However, it is worth noting that the levels of stigma in both groups reduced over time (with a significant reduction in negative appraisals of unusual experiences and a strong trend, P = 0.051, for an increase in perceived social acceptability). This suggests that a non-catastrophic, non-judgemental and empathic approach to engaging such individuals is unlikely to result in increased internalised stigma. This is consistent with data from qualitative interviews of trial participants, Reference Byrne and Morrison37 who consistently reported the benefits of normalising, destigmatising messages delivered directly by cognitive therapy and indirectly by regular monitoring of unusual experiences and beliefs. The latter appeared to occur as participants inferred from the regular questions about unusual experiences that many people must experience them and, therefore, they are not as unusual or different as they had originally believed. Reference Byrne and Morrison37 Thus, active monitoring within a normalising framework may also be beneficial, and would be benign, easy to implement and consistent with International Early Psychosis Association guidelines regarding treatment in the at-risk phase. 38

There are several other clinical implications of this study. Cognitive therapy reduces negative appraisals of psychotic experiences, which should provide direct benefit given the toxic nature of internalised stigma. Cognitive therapy aims to encourage a normalising, non-catastrophic perspective on the understanding of psychotic experiences within a collaborative framework, which may be the mechanism by which such negative appraisals are reduced. It may be that targeting the social acceptability of such experiences more directly, using methods derived from the literature on prevention of stigma and discrimination (such as contact with other service users, identification of positive role models and celebration of difference), may result in greater effects. However, it is also important to consider that, until wider societal views of psychosis and mental health problems are improved, a cautious approach to disclosure of such experiences may be appropriate. Thus, it is essential that any individual approaches, such as cognitive therapy, are delivered in a context that includes campaigns designed to influence the general public.

Funding

This research was supported by funding from the Medical Research Council (G0500264) and the Department of Health.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Yung, AR, McGorry, PD, McFarlane, C, Jackson, HJ, Patton, GC, Rakkar, A. Monitoring and care of young people at incipient risk of psychosis. Schizophr Bull 1996; 22: 283303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Yang, LH, Wonpat-Borja, AJ, Opler, MG, Corcoran, CM. Potential stigma associated with inclusion of the psychosis risk syndrome in the DSM-V: an empirical question. Schizophr Res 2010; 120: 42–8.Google Scholar
3 Corcoran, CM, First, MB, Cornblatt, BA. The psychosis risk syndrome and its proposed inclusion in the DSM-V: a risk/benefit analysis. Schizophr Res 2010; 120: 1622.Google Scholar
4 Morrison, AP, Byrne, RE, Bentall, RP. DSM-5 and the ‘Psychosis Risk Syndrome’: whose best interests would it serve? Psychosis 2010; 2: 96–9.Google Scholar
5 Addington, J, Epstein, I, Liu, L, French, P, Boydell, KM, Zipursky, RB. A randomized controlled trial of cognitive behavioral therapy for individuals at clinical high risk of psychosis. Schizophr Res 2011; 125: 5461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 McGlashan, TH, Zipursky, RB, Perkins, D, Addington, J, Miller, T, Woods, SW, et al Randomized, double-blind trial of olanzapine versus placebo in patients prodromally symptomatic for psychosis. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163: 790–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 McGorry, PD, Yung, AR, Phillips, LJ, Yuen, HP, Francey, S, Cosgrave, EM, et al Randomized controlled trial of interventions designed to reduce the risk of progression to first-episode psychosis in a clinical sample with subthreshold symptoms. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59: 921–8.Google Scholar
8 Morrison, AP, French, P, Walford, L, Lewis, SW, Kilcommons, A, Green, J, et al Cognitive therapy for the prevention of psychosis in people at ultra-high risk. Randomised controlled trial. Br J Psychiatry 2004; 185: 291–7.Google Scholar
9 Amminger, GP, Schafer, MR, Papageorgiou, K, Klier, CM, Cotton, SM, Harrigan, SM, et al Long-chain omega-3 fatty acids for indicated prevention of psychotic disorders: a randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67: 146–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10 Bechdolf, A, Wagner, M, Ruhrmann, S, Harrigan, S, Putzfeld, V, Pukrop, R, et al Preventing progression to first-episode psychosis in early initial prodromal states. Br J Psychiatry 2012; 200: 22–9.Google Scholar
11 Yung, AR, Phillips, LJ, Nelson, B, Francey, SM, PanYuen, H, Simmons, MB, et al Randomized controlled trial of interventions for young people at ultra high risk for psychosis: 6-month analysis. J Clin Psychiatry 2011; 72: 430–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12 Morrison, AP, French, P, Stewart, S, Birchwood, M, Fowler, B, Gumley, AL. Early Detection and Intervention Evaluation for people at risk of psychosis (EDIE-2): a multisite randomised controlled trial of cognitive therapy for at risk mental states. BMJ 2012; 344: e2233.Google Scholar
13 Stafford, MR, Jackson, H, Mayo-Wilson, E, Morrison, AP, Kendall, T. Early interventions to prevent psychosis: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ 2013; 346: f185.Google Scholar
14 Warner, R. The prevention of schizophrenia: what interventions are safe and effective? Schizophr Bull 2001; 27: 551–62.Google Scholar
15 Corcoran, C, Malaspina, D, Hercher, L. Prodromal interventions for schizophrenia vulnerability: the risks of being “at risk”. Schizophr Res 2005; 73: 173–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16 French, P, Morrison, AP. Early Detection and Cognitive Therapy for People at High Risk of Developing Psychosis: A Treatment Approach. Wiley, 2004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
17 Bentall, RP, Morrison, AP. More harm than good: the case against using antipsychotic drugs to prevent severe mental illness. J Ment Health 2002; 11: 351–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
18 Link, BG, Yang, LH, Phelan, JC, Collins, PY. Measuring mental illness stigma. Schizophr Bull 2004; 30: 511–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19 Corrigan, PW, Watson, AC. The paradox of self-stigma and mental illness. Clin Psychol 2002; 9: 3553.Google Scholar
20 Ritsher, JB, Otilingam, PG, Grajales, M. Internalized stigma of mental illness: psychometric properties of a new measure. Psychiatry Res 2003; 121: 3149.Google Scholar
21 Yung, AR, Yuen, HP, McGorry, PD, Phillips, LJ, Kelly, D, Dell'Olio, M, et al Mapping the onset of psychosis – the comprehensive assessment of at risk mental states (CAARMS). Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2005; 39: 964–71.Google Scholar
22 Birchwood, M, Mason, R, MacMillan, F, Healy, J. Depression, demoralization and control over psychotic illness: a comparison of depressed and nondepressed patients with a chronic psychosis. Psychol Med 1993; 23: 387–95.Google Scholar
23 Birchwood, M, Jackson, C, Brunet, K, Holden, J, Barton, K. Personal Beliefs about Illness Questionnaire-Revised (PBIQ-R): reliability and validation in a first episode sample. Br J Clin Psychology 2012; 51: 448–58.Google Scholar
24 Morrison, AP. The interpretation of intrusions in psychosis: an integrative cognitive approach to hallucinations and delusions. Behav Cogn Psychother 2001; 29: 257–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
25 Little, RJA, Rubin, DB. Statistical Analysis with Missing Data. John Wiley and Sons, 2002.Google Scholar
26 Maracy, M, Dunn, G. Estimating dose-response effects in psychological treatment trials: the role of instrumental variables. Stat Methods Med Res 2011; 20: 191215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27 Morrison, AP, Stewart, S, French, P, Bentall, RP, Birchwood, M, Byrne, R, et al Early Detection and Intervention Evaluation for people at high-risk of psychosis-2 (EDIE-2): trial rationale, design and baseline characteristics. Early Interv Psychiatry 2011; 5: 2432.Google Scholar
28 Carpenter, WT. Anticipating DSM-V: should psychosis risk become a diagnostic class? Schizophr Bull 2009; 35: 841–3.Google Scholar
29 Thornicroft, G, Brohan, E, Rose, D, Sartorius, N, Leese, M. Global pattern of experienced and anticipated discrimination against people with schizophrenia: a cross-sectional survey. Lancet 2009; 373: 408–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30 Goffman, E. Stigma: Notes on the Management of Spoiled Identity. Penguin Books, 1963.Google Scholar
31 Brohan, E, Elgie, R, Sartorius, N, Thornicroft, G. Self-stigma, empowerment and perceived discrimination among people with schizophrenia in 14 European countries: the GAMIAN-Europe study. Schizophr Res 2010; 122: 232–8.Google Scholar
32 Thornicroft, G. Most people with mental illness are not treated. Lancet 2007; 370: 807–8.Google Scholar
33 Thornicroft, G, Rose, D, Kassam, A. Discrimination in health care against people with mental illness. Int Rev Psychiatry 2007; 19: 113–22.Google Scholar
34 Corrigan, PW, Watson, AC, Barr, L. The self-stigma of mental illness: implications for self-esteem and self-efficacy. J Soc Clin Psychol 2006; 25: 875–84.Google Scholar
35 Birchwood, M, Trower, P, Brunet, K, Gilbert, P, Iqbal, Z, Jackson, C. Social anxiety and the shame of psychosis: a study in first episode psychosis. Behav Res Ther 2007; 45: 1025–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36 Livingston, JD, Boyd, JE. Correlates and consequences of internalized stigma for people living with mental illness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Soc Sci Med 2010; 71: 2150–61.Google Scholar
37 Byrne, R, Morrison, AP. Young people at risk of psychosis: their subjective experiences of monitoring and cognitive behaviour therapy in the EDIE 2 trial. Psychol Psychother, in press.Google Scholar
38 International Early Psychosis Association Writing Group. International clinical practice guidelines for early psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 2005; 187 (suppl 48): s1204.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Baseline characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2 Means and standard deviations for the negative appraisals and social acceptability subscales of the Personal Beliefs about Experiences Questionnaire

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.