Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-r7xzm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T08:32:57.682Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Childhood sleep disturbance and risk of psychotic experiences at 18: UK birth cohort

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

A. Thompson*
Affiliation:
Division of Mental Health and Wellbeing, University of Warwick, Coventry
S. T. Lereya
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Warwick, Coventry
G. Lewis
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University College London, London
S. Zammit
Affiliation:
MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University, Cardiff and Centre for Academic Mental Health, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol
H. L. Fisher
Affiliation:
MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, London
D. Wolke
Affiliation:
Division of Mental Health and Wellbeing and Department of Psychology, University of Warwick, Coventry, UK
*
Andrew Thompson, Division of Mental Health and Wellbeing, Warwick Medical School, University of Warwick, Coventry, CV47AL, UK. Email: Andrew.d.thompson@warwick.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Sleep disturbances are commonly reported in the psychosis prodrome, but rarely explored in relation to psychotic experiences.

Aims

To investigate the relationship between specific parasomnias (nightmares, night terrors and sleepwalking) in childhood and later adolescent psychotic experiences.

Method

The sample comprised 4720 individuals from a UK birth cohort. Mothers reported on children's experience of regular nightmares at several time points between 2 and 9 years. Experience of nightmares, night terrors and sleepwalking was assessed using a semi-structured interview at age 12. Psychotic experiences were assessed at ages 12 and 18 using a semi-structured clinical interview.

Results

There was a significant association between the presence of nightmares at 12 and psychotic experiences at 18 when adjusted for possible confounders and psychotic experiences at 12 (OR = 1.62, 95% CI 1.19–2.20). The odds ratios were larger for those who reported persistent psychotic experiences.

Conclusions

The presence of nightmares might be an early risk indicator for psychosis.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2015

In recent years there has been particular interest in the significance of psychotic experiences in the general population. The non-trivial rates of these experiences in non-clinical populations Reference Kelleher, Connor, Clarke, Devlin, Harley and Cannon1Reference Zammit, Kounali, Cannon, David, Gunnell and Heron3 has led to the re-emergence of a continuum model of psychosis. Reference van Os, Hanssen, Bijl and Ravelli4 Although recent research has suggested that the relationship between established risk factors for schizophrenia and psychotic experiences may be weaker than initially thought, Reference Horwood, Salvi, Thomas, Duffy, Gunnell and Hollis2,Reference Zammit, Horwood, Thompson, Thomas, Menezes and Gunnell5 the relevance of these subclinical experiences to early intervention strategies in psychosis remains important. Reference Kelleher, Lynch, Harley, Molloy, Roddy and Fitzpatrick6,Reference Fisher, Caspi, Poulton, Meier, Houts and Harrington7 Therefore researchers have continued to investigate risk factors and possible causal mechanisms for the development of these psychotic experiences. One such potential risk factor in children that can readily be identified by parents, children and professionals is disturbance of sleep. Early researchers were interested particularly in the boundaries between nightmares and psychotic phenomena Reference Jung8 and some have proposed dreams and nightmares as a potential model for psychotic phenomena. Reference Scarone, Manzone, Gambini, Kantzas, Limosani and D'Agostino9 Specific difficulties with sleep quality Reference Haffmans, Hoencamp, Knegtering and van Heycop ten Ham10 , sleep–wake rhythm Reference Wulff, Dijk, Middleton, Foster and Joyce11 and electrophysiological sleep architecture (such as sleep spindles) Reference Wilson and Argyropoulos12 are commonly reported in patients with schizophrenia. Moreover, sleep disturbance is one of the commonest symptoms retrospectively reported by patients with schizophrenia as part of a prodrome of the disorder Reference Yung and McGorry13 and has been shown to be of particular value in predicting development of psychosis in clinical high-risk individuals. Reference Ruhrmann, Schultze-Lutter, Salokangas, Heinimaa, Linszen and Dingemans14 We are aware of only one study that found that sleep disturbance reported by children attending a psychiatric clinic was associated with increased odds of a later diagnosis of schizophrenia; Reference Cannon, Walsh and Hollis15 we know of no prospective data on the relationship between sleep and psychotic experiences.

The relationship between specific sleep disturbances such as parasomnias and psychosis would seem particularly important for a number of reasons. Some parasomnias such as hypnagogic and hypnopompic hallucinations are phenomenologically similar to hallucinations in the waking state. Other common parasomnias such as nightmares and night terrors have often been associated with psychopathology. Reference Levin and Fireman16,Reference Ohayon, Guilleminault and Priest17 Cross-sectional studies have also suggested that nightmares are related to psychosis proneness or schizotypy. Reference Claridge, Clark and Davis18,Reference Levin19

We have previously demonstrated a cross-sectional relationship between the presence of nightmares and night terrors and psychotic experiences at age 12. Reference Fisher, Lereya, Thompson, Lewis, Zammit and Wolke20 However, it is essential to determine whether parasomnias are possible precursors of psychotic experiences using longitudinal data. Therefore, we examined the relationship between the most common parasomnias in childhood, namely nightmares, night terrors and sleepwalking, reported both in early childhood and at the age of 12 to later psychotic experiences reported at 18 years using data from a large UK birth cohort. It was hypothesised that all types of parasomnia would be associated with both the development and persistence of psychotic experiences during late adolescence.

Method

Participants

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is a birth cohort study, set in the UK, examining the determinants of development, health and disease during childhood and beyond. Reference Boyd, Golding, Macleod, Lawlor, Fraser and Henderson21,Reference Fraser, Macdonald-Wallis, Tilling, Boyd, Golding and Davey Smith22 Briefly, women who were resident in Avon, UK while pregnant, and had an expected delivery date between 1 April 1991 and 31 December 1992 were approached to participate in the study, leading to the inclusion of 14 775 live births with 14 701 of these alive at 1 year of age. From the first trimester of pregnancy parents completed postal questionnaires about themselves and the study child's health and development. Children were invited to attend annual assessment clinics, including face-to-face interviews, psychological and physical tests from 7 years of age onward. Please note that the study website contains details of all of the data that is available through a fully searchable data dictionary (http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary/). This analysis is based on 4720 children who completed the Psychosis-Like Symptoms semi-structured interview (PLIKSi) at approximately 18 years of age. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the local research ethics committees and informed consent was provided by all participants.

Outcome variables

Psychotic experiences at 18

The PLIKSi Reference Horwood, Salvi, Thomas, Duffy, Gunnell and Hollis2,Reference Zammit, Kounali, Cannon, David, Gunnell and Heron3 is a semi-structured face-to-face interview conducted in private with the study member. The PLIKSi comprises 12 core questions eliciting key psychotic experiences covering hallucinations (visual and auditory), delusions (spied on, persecution, thoughts read, reference, control, grandiosity and other unspecified delusions) and experiences of thought interference (thought broadcasting, insertion and withdrawal). For these core items, seven stem questions were derived from the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children-IV (DISC-IV) Reference Shaffer, Fisher, Lucas, Dulcan and Schwab-Stone23 and five from the Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry version 2.0 (SCAN 2.0). 24 Cross-questioning was used to establish the presence of symptoms since 12 years of age, and coding followed the glossary definitions and rating rules for SCAN. The interviewers were psychology graduates trained in the use of the SCAN psychosis section and the PLIKSi. Items were assessed as not present, suspected or definitely present. The average kappa value for interrater reliability of rating psychotic experiences was 0.83 and the kappa for test–retest reliability was 0.76. Reference Zammit, Kounali, Cannon, David, Gunnell and Heron3 Positive responses were probed for additional information including whether the experiences could be attributed solely to sleep or fever. Further detail on the instrument is available elsewhere. Reference Zammit, Kounali, Cannon, David, Gunnell and Heron3 The main PLIKSi outcome was the presence of one or more of the PLIKSi experiences suspected or definitely present at age 18. In order to rule out the possibility that any psychotic experiences associated with the parasomnias were only those that occurred when a child was falling asleep (hypnagogic), waking up (hypnopompic) or had a fever, only psychotic experiences that were not attributed to sleep or fever were used.

Incident and persistent psychotic experiences

The PLIKSi was also conducted with study members when they were 12 years of age Reference Horwood, Salvi, Thomas, Duffy, Gunnell and Hollis2 to establish psychotic experiences occurring during the 6 months prior to interview. Suspected or definite psychotic experiences were identified with an average kappa value for interrater reliability of 0.72. Reference Horwood, Salvi, Thomas, Duffy, Gunnell and Hollis2 Using these data a further set of PLIKSi outcomes were created: (a) none – no psychotic experiences reported at age 12 or age 18; (b) incident – suspected or definite psychotic experiences present at 18 years but not present at 12 years; (c) remitted – suspected or definite psychotic experiences present at 12 years but not present at 18 years; and (d) persistent – suspected or definite psychotic experiences present at both 12 and 18 years. Again, these were excluding those experiences attributable to sleep or fever to remove this potential source of confounding.

Predictor variables

Persistent nightmares during preschool and school

Persistent nightmares during preschool and school were derived from postal questionnaires completed by mothers when the children were aged 2.5, 3.5, 4.75, 5.75, 6.75 and 9 years. Specifically, mothers were asked: ‘In the past year, has your child regularly had nightmares?’ The presence of nightmares at one time point, two time points and three or more time points v. no nightmares was considered here in order to assess the persistence of nightmares during the preschool and school period. Children were excluded from the analysis if they did not have nightmare data available for at least three time points. Other sleep disturbances during preschool and school at these time points (difficulty falling asleep and night waking) were also available in the cohort but were not associated with any measure of psychotic experiences at 12 (see Fisher et al Reference Fisher, Lereya, Thompson, Lewis, Zammit and Wolke25 ) or psychotic experiences at 18 and were not the focus of this paper.

Sleeping problems at 12 years

Sleeping problems at 12 years were assessed via face-to-face semi-structured interviews with the children when they were aged on average 12.9 years old. The interviews were conducted by psychology graduates who were trained to a gold standard and attended a monthly clinic with the study's consultant psychiatrists to obtain consensus on difficult to rate cases. Children were asked a series of questions concerning sleeping problems (nightmares, night terrors and sleepwalking) during the 6 months prior to the interview based on DSM-IV 26 criteria for sleeping disorders (for more information see Fisher et al Reference Fisher, Lereya, Thompson, Lewis, Zammit and Wolke20 ). Positive responses were probed to obtain further information to distinguish between nightmares and night terrors (for example when they occurred at night, amnesia upon awakening etc.). Reference Wolke, Rutter and Hay27 An overall rating of not present (0), suspected (1) or definitely (2) present was then made for each type of parasomnia based on all the information obtained. ‘Definitely present’ was rated when all criteria for a specific parasomnia were fulfilled and ‘suspected’ when most but not all criteria were fulfilled after probing. The average kappa value for interrater reliability was 0.72. Sleeping problems coded overall as suspected or definitely present were used in the current analysis. Frequency of these experiences was measured as: only once or twice, quite often (monthly), often (almost weekly), most of the time (most days) and several times per night. Because of the number of responses this was simplified into three categories: none; only once or twice; and quite often, often or most of the time. Another variable was derived (any parasomnia) to reflect whether the children had reported any of these particular parasomnia symptoms (nightmare and/or night terror and/or sleepwalking) at 12 years of age.

Confounders

The following confounders were considered.

  1. (a) IQ: an abbreviated form of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III (UK version) Reference Wechsler28 was used to derive an overall IQ for the children who attended the ALSPAC clinic at age 8.

  2. (b) Family adversity: multiple family risk factors were assessed during pregnancy with the Family Adversity Index (FAI). Reference Bowen, Heron, Waylen and Wolke29

  3. (c) Psychiatric disorders: DSM-IV psychiatric diagnoses were made at 7 years using the Development and Well-Being Assessment (DAWBA) Reference Goodman, Ford, Richards, Gatward and Meltzer30 based on parent and teacher reports. (d) Anxiety: DSM-IV diagnosis of an anxiety disorder at age 10 was made using the DAWBA Reference Goodman, Ford, Richards, Gatward and Meltzer30 based on parental reports.

  4. (e) Depression: symptoms were assessed using the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ) Reference Angold, Costello, Messer and Pickles31 administered at 10 (child report), 11.6 (mother report) and 12.6 (child report) years. The total score at each time point was collapsed into a dichotomous variable according to previously identified cut-points (scores of <11 indicated non-clinical symptoms, whereas scores of ⩾11 indicated clinically relevant depressive symptoms). Reference Angold, Erkanli, Silberg, Eaves and Costello32

  5. (f) Child abuse: mothers reported via a series of postal questionnaires on whether their child had been physically hurt by someone and also whether their child had been sexually abused. Questionnaires were completed when the children were aged 2.5, 3.5, 4.8, 5.8 and 6.8 years. A positive response to either of these two questions at any of the time points was considered to indicate physical or sexual abuse.

  6. (g) Enuresis: mothers reported via postal questionnaires on their child's bedwetting when the child was aged 4.5, 5.5, 6.5, 7.5 and 9 years. Reports of bedwetting for at least 1 week at any of these time points or still wearing a nappy at night at 5.5 years were taken to indicate the presence of enuresis.

Statistical analyses

Selective drop out was determined by comparing those who completed the PLIKSi at age 18 with those who did not, using binary logistic regression in the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 21. Analyses were repeated with the data weighted using inverse probability (of having missing outcome data) weights to account for those lost to follow-up. To assess whether persistence of nightmares during the preschool or school years or parasomnias at age 12 were associated with psychotic experiences at age 18, three sets of logistic regression analyses were conducted. Model A reported unadjusted analyses, model B controlled for the confounders described above and model C controlled for all confounders plus suspected/definite psychotic experiences without attributions at age 12. Models A and B were repeated for the four-category persistence variables using multinomial regression. All analyses were conducted on individuals for whom all variables were available (outcomes, predictors and confounders).

Results

Differences between participants with and without PLIKSi

Differences in sociodemographic, family environment and child characteristics for ALSPAC participants with and without the PLIKSi at 18 years are shown in online Table DS1. Those who completed the PLIKSi were more often girls, came from less adverse family environments and were more likely to be born to mothers with a higher education level and living with partners in mortgaged properties. These children were also more likely to have a higher IQ at age 8 and less likely to have a DSM-IV psychiatric diagnosis at age 7. However, children who attended the PLIKSi at age 18 were more likely to be depressed, to have been physically or sexually abused and have experienced more enuresis and persistent nightmares during preschool and school. No differences were found for children's sleeping problems (nightmares, night-terrors, sleepwalking or any parasomnia) or their psychotic experiences at age 12. In this study, we did not use any statistical method for imputing missing values but did repeat the analyses with data weighted to account for the selective drop out and did not find substantially different results (see online Tables DS2 and DS3). This study reports the results only for children who completed the PLIKSi at 18 years of age (n = 4720; 2666 girls, 56.5%).

Prevalence of psychotic experiences and sleep problems

At 18 years, 374 (7.9%) from the entire sample of 4720 were judged to have one or more suspected or definite psychotic experiences not attributable to sleep or fever. Not all study members who had psychotic experiences data at 18 had data at 12. Out of 4060 study members with data at both time points, 190 study members (4.7%) had suspected or definite psychotic experiences without any attributes only at age 18 (incident) and 100 (2.5%) had psychotic experiences at both 12 and 18 years (persistent). Additionally, 370 (9.1%) study members had suspected or definite psychotic experiences without any attributes only at age 12 (remitted).

Mothers of 1019 children (25.1%) from the sample of 4060 never reported any nightmares for their children, 830 (20.4%) reported children having regular nightmares at one time point, 708 (17.4%) reported children having nightmares at two time points and 1503 (37.0%) reported children having nightmares at three or more time points between 2.5 and 9 years. At age 12, 1010 (24.9%) children reported having nightmares in the previous 6 months, 359 (8.8%) reported having night terrors and 513 (12.6%) reported sleepwalking in the past 6 months. Overall at 12 years, 1473 (36.3%) children reported experiencing at least one type of parasomnia.

Parasomnias and psychotic experiences at 18

Prospective associations between childhood nightmares, parasomnias at age 12 and age 18 psychotic experiences are shown in Table 1. Persistent childhood nightmares, and any parasomnias, nightmares and night terrors at age 12 were associated with suspected/definite psychotic experiences at age 18 excluding those attributable to sleep and fever. However, once psychotic experiences at age 12 were controlled for, the associations for persistent childhood nightmares and night terrors were attenuated and just failed to reach conventional levels of statistical significance. Moreover, no association was found for age 12 sleepwalking. There does appear to be a relationship between increasing frequency of nightmares and night terrors and psychotic experiences demonstrated by increasing odds ratios at greater frequencies (online Tables DS4 and DS5), but the numbers in the analysis are small and these results should be interpreted with caution.

Table 1 Sleeping problems in childhood and suspected/definite psychotic experiences at 18 years excluding attributions to sleep and fever a

Psychotic experiences, OR (95% CI)
n Model A b Model B c Model C d
Persistent nightmares, 2.5–9 years 3036
 Linear trend 1.19 (1.06–1.35) 1.16 (1.03–1.32) 1.13 (0.99–1.28)
Nightmares, 12 years 3057
 No Reference Reference Reference
 Yes 2.14 (1.59–2.87) 1.92 (1.42–2.58) 1.62 (1.19–2.20)
Night terrors, 12 years 3056
 No Reference Reference Reference
 Yes 1.93 (1.28–2.91) 1.71 (1.13–2.60) 1.52 (0.99–2.34)
Sleepwalking, 12 years 3056
 No Reference Reference Reference
 Yes 1.24 (0.83–1.84) 1.19 (0.79–1.78) 1.11 (0.73–1.67)
Any parasomnias, 12 years 3053
 No Reference Reference Reference
 Yes 2.00 (1.50–2.66) 1.82 (1.36–2.43) 1.60 (1.19–2.16)

a. The reference group in all of the analysis is made up of participants without any sleeping problems. Any parasomnia refers to reports of any nightmares, night terrors or sleepwalking at age 12.

b. Unadjusted analysis.

c. Adjusted for gender, Family Adversity Index (FAI) score, enuresis at 4.5, 5.5, 6.5, 7.5 or 9 years, IQ at 8 years, depression score at 10, 11.6 or 12.6 years, sexual or physical abuse at 2.5, 3.5, 4.8, 5.8 or 6.8 years, Development and Well-being Assessment (DAWBA) score at 7 years (any Axis I diagnoses) and any anxiety disorder diagnosis at 10 years.

d. Adjusted for gender, FAI score, enuresis at 4.5, 5, 5.5, 6.5, 7.5 or 9 years, IQ at 8 years, depression score at 10, 11.6 or 12.6 years, sexual or physical abuse at 2.5, 3.5, 4.8, 5.8 or 6.8 years, DAWBA score at 7 years (any Axis I diagnoses), any anxiety disorder diagnosis at 10 years and psychotic experiences at age 12 without attributes to sleep and fever.

Parasomnias and persistence of psychotic experiences

Persistent childhood nightmares were significantly associated with new incidences of suspected/definite psychotic experiences at age 18 without attributions to sleep and fever (Table 2). However, the odds ratio was slightly attenuated and the confidence intervals included the null when all the confounders were added into the model. Nightmares and any parasomnias at age 12 were significantly associated with new incidences of suspected/definite psychotic experiences at age 18. All of the parasomnias were significantly associated with remitted psychotic experiences (psychotic experiences present only at age 12). Adjustment for potential confounders only slightly attenuated the associations. Persistent childhood nightmares, nightmares and night terrors at age 12 and any parasomnia at age 12 were significantly associated with persistent psychotic experiences (psychotic experiences present at both age 12 and 18) but sleepwalking at age 12 was not associated. Adjusting for potential confounders reduced the associations slightly.

Table 2 Childhood sleeping problems and persistence of suspected/definite psychotic experiences between 12 and 18 years excluding attributions to sleep and fever a

Unadjusted analyses, b OR (95% CI) Adjusted analyses, c OR (95% CI)
n None Incident
(at 18 but
not 12)
Remitted
(at 12 but
not 18)
Persistent
(at 12 and 18)
None Incident
(at 18 but
not 12)
Remitted
(at 12 but
not 18)
Persistent
(at 12 and 18)
Nightmares, 2–9 years 3056 Reference 1.18(1.02–1.36) 1.25 (1.12–1.39) 1.32 (1.06–1.65) Reference 1.15 (0.99–1.33) 1.21 (1.08–1.35) 1.27 (1.02–1.60)
Nightmares, 12 years 3057 Reference 1.83 (1.28–2.63) 2.85 (2.20–3.69) 4.31 (2.59–7.17) Reference 1.69 (1.17–2.44) 2.71 (2.08–3.53) 3.87 (2.30–6.51)
Night terrors, 12 years 3056 Reference 1.65 (0.98–2.80) 1.90 (1.30–2.77) 3.14 (1.68–5.87) Reference 1.52 (0.89–2.60) 1.77 (1.20–2.61) 2.70 (1.42–5.15)
Sleepwalking, 12 years 3056 Reference 1.35 (0.84–2.16) 1.65 (1.19–2.31) 1.22 (0.59–2.49) Reference 1.33 (0.83–2.14) 1.63 (1.16–2.28) 1.12 (0.54–2.31)
Any parasomnias, 12 years 3053 Reference 1.81 (1.29–2.54) 2.38 (1.84–3.07) 3.38 (2.01–5.70) Reference 1.70 (1.21–2.41) 2.27 (1.75–2.95) 2.94(1.72–5.02)

a. The reference group in all of the analysis is made up of participants without any psychotic experiences at 12 or 18 years. Any parasomnias refers to any reports of nightmares, night terrors or sleepwalking.

b. Unadjusted analysis.

c. Adjusted for gender, Family Adversity Index score, enuresis at 4.5, 5.5, 6.5, 7.5 or 9 years, IQ at 8 years, depression score at 10, 11.6 or 12.6 years, sexual or physical abuse at 2.5, 3.5, 4.8, 5.8 or 6.8 years, Development and Well-being Assessment score at 7 years (any Axis 1 diagnoses) and any anxiety disorder diagnosis at 10 years.

Although in our sample parasomnias at 12 were more common in those with persistent psychotic experiences between 12 and 18 compared with those whose psychotic experiences had remitted at 18, numbers were small and there was no evidence that this difference was more than that resulting from random error/chance (nightmares: OR = 1.51, 95% CI 0.87–1.64; night terrors: OR = 1.65, 95% CI 0.82–3.34; any parasomnia: OR = 1.43, 95% CI 0.81–2.52).

Discussion

Summary of the results

Data from this large UK birth cohort suggest that those who have childhood experiences of nightmares and night terrors at age 12 are more likely to report psychotic experiences at age 18. However, the relationship for night terrors was attenuated when a number of potential confounders were controlled for including age, IQ, childhood abuse, family adversity, early mood disturbance and also baseline psychotic experiences at age 12. The relationship with nightmares and subsequent psychotic experiences was also observed in measures of nightmares collected in early childhood although this relationship was attenuated when controlling for possible confounders.

Comparison with previous studies

The study confirms our previous finding of a cross-sectional relationship between nightmares and night terrors and psychotic experiences at age 12 but not sleepwalking. Reference Fisher, Lereya, Thompson, Lewis, Zammit and Wolke20 However, in this study the relationship with night terrors was weaker. It also extends the longitudinal findings of this study in that early childhood nightmares are associated with the development of psychotic symptoms both at age 12 and up to the later age of 18 but again the strength of the association is somewhat weaker. Other groups have examined the relationship between sleep and the broader concepts of schizotypy or psychosis proneness and found a similar relationship with nightmares. Reference Levin19,Reference Fisher, Lereya, Thompson, Lewis, Zammit and Wolke20 Indeed, Levin and colleagues concluded that ‘nightmare experience may be a useful conjoint behavioural indicator for the early detection of schizophrenia-spectrum psychopathology’. Reference Levin19 However, these studies have either been cross-sectional or used much smaller samples than this study. There have been no previous studies examining night terrors or sleepwalking and psychotic experiences longitudinally to our knowledge so these are novel findings. The association with types of parasomnia and psychotic experiences is similar to the findings of Koffel & Watson Reference Koffel and Watson33 who reported associations between unusual sleeping problems (including nightmares, vivid dreaming, narcolepsy symptoms and complex night-time behaviours) and schizotypy but not for other sleep disturbances (for example insomnia, lassitude/fatigue). However, this comparison is limited by the fact that we did not comprehensively assess insomnia or other types of routine sleep disturbance.

Strengths and weaknesses

The strengths of the study are the longitudinal nature of the data collection, the large sample size, the comprehensive approach to eliciting psychotic experiences and the ability to adjust for a number of potential confounders in the relationship between sleep disturbances and psychotic experiences. We were also able to exclude possible hypnagogic and hypnopompic hallucinations from our measure of psychotic experiences. However, there are a number of potential weaknesses of the study worth noting. We could not exclude psychotic experiences at 18 attributed to substance use. We were unable to exclude the presence of other parasomnias that may mimic nightmares/night terrors such as obstructive sleep apnoea and nocturnal epilepsy and we did not exclude individuals with other chronic organic conditions that could cause parasomnias, although this number is likely to be low in a healthy birth cohort. We did not have a satisfactory rating of insomnia in the cohort and measures of this and sleep quality would provide a more comprehensive picture of sleep difficulties as specific risk factors for psychotic symptoms. The rating of parasomnia symptoms at 12 was based on a semi-structured interview eliciting an example of the phenomenon and detailed probing. However, the prevalence of night terrors (8.8%) and sleepwalking (12.6%) found in the current sample at age 12 was higher than has previously been reported for children of a similar age. These types of parasomnia tend to become less frequent by late childhood and a study conducted by Laberge et al Reference Laberge, Tremblay, Vitaro and Montplaisir34 of a representative sample of children from Canada found that only 2.3% of them were still experiencing night terrors and only 6.8% were sleepwalking by 12 years of age. One possible reason for the discrepancy is that the current study utilised the young adolescent as the data source of sleeping difficulties whereas the Laberge et al Reference Laberge, Tremblay, Vitaro and Montplaisir34 study used parental reports. The parents of the young adolescents in the Canadian sample may not have been aware of any sleeping problems if they were not woken at the time. Similarly, many parents of infants are not aware of their child waking if they do not cry and alert the parents. Reference St James-Roberts35 There is a possibility that the self-report of night terrors and sleepwalking especially (where there is classically little memory for the event) may be subject to error. However, there is some evidence that the correlation between self-report and informant report for sleep difficulties in children is good Reference Owens, Maxim, Nobile, McGuinn and Msall36 and our assessment was using a semi-structured interview with raters trained to a gold standard and achieving a good interrater reliability on these items. The results of the relationship between parent reports of early experience of nightmares are also broadly similar to the child report at 12. However, we would encourage replication of the results at age 12 using a combination of child report and parental report measures.

We also have limited information on the impact of the sleeping difficulties at age 12 and are therefore unable to comment on the role of distress in relation to these events in respect of psychotic experiences. There is also the issue of selective drop out from the original cohort. Nevertheless, empirical simulations demonstrate that even when drop out is correlated with predictor/confounder variables, the relationship between predictors and outcome is unlikely to substantially be altered by selective drop-out processes. Reference Wolke, Waylen, Samara, Steer, Goodman and Ford37 Indeed, when the analyses were repeated after weighting for this selective drop out the results were very similar (online Tables DS1 and DS2). Although the results are only slightly attenuated by potential confounders in the relationship between sleep symptoms and psychotic experiences, we cannot be sure that we have accounted for all residual confounding factors. We are also aware we had limited power to investigate the question of whether those reporting persistent psychotic experiences were more likely to report earlier parasomnia symptoms compared with those whose psychotic experiences had remitted at 18.

Possible mechanisms

Assuming our method of excluding those psychotic experiences that are related to falling asleep or waking did accurately exclude those individuals who had hypnopompic and hypnagogic hallucinations, then there are a number of possible explanations as to why nightmares in particular might be related to psychotic experiences to consider. These theories need to take into account that the relationship seems specific for nightmares and to some extent night terrors and not sleepwalking so it is not a general effect of this type of sleep disturbance. First, the boundaries between wake and sleep especially in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep may be important in relation to sleep and psychotic experiences. One particular hypothesis, for example is that the boundaries between being asleep and awake may be too ‘flexible’ in some individuals with psychosis resulting in brief lapses into REM sleep (within which nightmares are experienced) occurring while they are awake, resulting in the experience of hallucinations during the day. Reference Mahowald and Schenck38 Conversely, slipping into a waking state while in non-REM sleep is postulated to account for the occurrence of night terrors and sleepwalking. However, although some studies have found poor sleep/wake boundaries among individuals with psychotic symptoms, Reference Howland39 there is little evidence of intrusions of REM sleep into waking states in patients with schizophrenia experiencing hallucinations. Reference Rechtschaffen, Schulsinger and Mednick40 It is difficult to explain the results with regards to sleepwalking using this model and even if intrusions into REM sleep and not non-REM sleep are particularly important this fails to explain the results for night terrors, which results in intrusions into non-REM sleep. Similarly, dissociation, which is often seen as a common mechanism for both development of psychotic symptoms and parasomnias Reference Laberge, Tremblay, Vitaro and Montplaisir34 would not appear to adequately explain the negative finding with sleepwalking.

It has been suggested that there may be shared patterns of neuroanatomical, neurochemical and neurophysiological pathways occurring in nightmares and the positive symptoms of psychosis, for example, the finding that cortical dopamine levels are raised during nightmares Reference D'Agostino, Limosani and Scarone41 and the functional significance of sleep spindles in psychosis Reference Keshavan, Montrose, Miewald and Jindal42 that are consistently reduced in schizophrenia. Reference Ferrarelli, Peterson, Sarasso, Riedner, Murphy and Benca43 Some studies have also reported a continuity between dreams and psychotic experiences; with overlapping content Reference D'Agostino, Aletti, Carboni, Cavallotti, Limosani and Manzone44 and indistinct barriers between these experiences. Reference Schredl45 This is related to the increased interest in dreams Reference Gottesmann46 or REM sleep Reference Gottesmann and Gottesman47 as a neurobiological model for schizophrenia or psychotic phenomena.

The presence of anxiety and depressive symptoms as confounding factors in those with sleep disturbance could potentially explain the findings. Higher levels of anxiety appear to be related to nightmares and night terrors but less strongly to sleepwalking, Reference Laberge, Tremblay, Vitaro and Montplaisir34 although these are still related in some patients. Reference Lopez, Jaussent, Scholz, Bayard, Montplaisir and Dauvilliers48 Anxiety symptoms may also be related to development of psychotic symptoms. Reference Hartley, Barrowclough and Haddock49 However, the adjustment for anxiety symptoms at age 10 and depression at 10, 11 and 12 did not significantly affect our results making this explanation less likely.

The relationship between traumatic experiences and parasomnia symptoms and psychotic experiences could be particularly important. Experience of stressful events has been related to both the development of nightmares Reference Terr, Bloch, Michel, Shi, Reinhardt and Metayer50 and psychotic symptoms in late childhood. Reference Schreier, Wolke, Thomas, Horwood, Hollis and Gunnell51,Reference Fisher, Schreier, Zammit, Maughan, Munafò and Lewis52 Nightmares are a diagnostic feature of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD, DSM-5 53 ) and there may be some specificity of nightmares to PTSD compared with other sleep disturbances. Reference Neylan, Marmar, Metzler, Weiss, Zatzick and Delucchi54 Therefore, the relationship could be as a result of an epiphenomenon of the experience of trauma, or trauma could be causing both experiences and could be a marker of the impact of the traumatic experience. A neurocognitive model of nightmares postulates a problem with fear extinction in those who experience nightmares Reference Nielsen and Levin55 and it may, for example, represent a shared mechanism for the production of particular psychotic symptoms. The relationship between nightmares (especially) and night terrors and trauma Reference Hartman, Crisp, Sedgwick and Borrow56 may be stronger than for sleepwalking, although sleepwalking can also be associated with traumatic psychological events. Reference Lopez, Jaussent, Scholz, Bayard, Montplaisir and Dauvilliers48 Unfortunately, there is not sufficiently good data available on significant trauma in the cohort to allow detailed exploration of this relationship at present; we are aware that only one individual in the cohort had a diagnosis of PTSD made using the DAWBA at age 7 (this individual did not report any psychotic experiences) and the measures of physical and sexual abuse were collected at an early age, were broad measures and were parental report. However, when adjusted for early levels of sexual and physical abuse in the analysis, this did not substantially change the results.

Implications

The clinical relevance of these findings may relate to the way that early nightmares (especially) and night terrors are viewed and potentially addressed by professionals or carers. It is likely that in some individuals, nightmares and night terrors have little significance to later psychopathology, however in individuals with additional risks such as a family psychiatric history or a past trauma exposure by adults or peers, Reference Schreier, Wolke, Thomas, Horwood, Hollis and Gunnell51,Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Fisher, Polanczyk, Moffitt and Caspi57 such sleep problems may have greater significance and may also highlight other unnoticed psychopathology or trauma. There are also established brief evidence-based psychological treatments of nightmares such as imagery rehearsal therapy (IRT) Reference Krakow58 and some medications for specific indications Reference Aurora, Zak, Auerbach, Casey, Chowdhuri and Karippot59 that could be considered to reduce the potential impact of nightmares on future psychopathology. Further work is needed to understand the mechanisms and interplay of risk factors, especially the role of trauma, in the relationship between these sleep disturbances and psychotic experiences. Despite this, these results suggest that specific parasomnias such as nightmares in children are a potential risk indicator for the development of psychopathology such as psychotic experiences.

Funding

The UK Medical Research Council (MRC) and the Wellcome Trust (grant 092731) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. Data collection was partly funded by an MRC grant (G0701503). D.W. and S.T.L.'s work on this study was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) grant ES/K003593/1. H.L.F. is funded by an MRC Population Health Scientist fellowship (G1002366). G.L. is an NIHR Senior Investigator. None of the funding bodies had any further role in the collection, analysis or interpretation of data, the writing of this manuscript, or the decision to submit this manuscript for publication.

Acknowledgements

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses.

Footnotes

Joint senior authors.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Kelleher, I, Connor, D, Clarke, MC, Devlin, N, Harley, M, Cannon, M. Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in childhood and adolescence: a systematic review and meta-analysis of population-based studies. Psychol Med 2012; 42: 1857–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2 Horwood, J, Salvi, G, Thomas, K, Duffy, L, Gunnell, D, Hollis, C, et al. IQ and non-clinical psychotic symptoms in 12-year-olds: results from the ALSPAC birth cohort. Br J Psychiatry 2008; 193: 185–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3 Zammit, S, Kounali, D, Cannon, M, David, AS, Gunnell, D, Heron, J, et al. Psychotic experiences and psychotic disorders at age 18 in relation to psychotic experiences at age 12 in a longitudinal population-based cohort study. Am J Psychiatry 2013; 170: 742–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4 van Os, J, Hanssen, M, Bijl, RV, Ravelli, A. Strauss (1969) revisited: a psychosis continuum in the general population? Schizophr Res 2000; 45: 1120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Zammit, S, Horwood, J, Thompson, A, Thomas, K, Menezes, P, Gunnell, D, et al. Investigating if psychosis-like symptoms (PLIKS) are associated with family history of schizophrenia or paternal age in the ALSPAC birth cohort. Schizophr Res 2008; 104: 279–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 Kelleher, I, Lynch, F, Harley, M, Molloy, C, Roddy, S, Fitzpatrick, C, et al. Psychotic symptoms in adolescence index risk for suicidal behavior: findings from 2 population-based case-control clinical interview studies. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012; 69: 1277–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 Fisher, HL, Caspi, A, Poulton, R, Meier, MH, Houts, R, Harrington, H, et al. Specificity of childhood psychotic symptoms for predicting schizophrenia by 38 years of age: a birth cohort study. Psychol Med 2013; 43: 2077–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Jung, C. On the psychogenesis of schizophrenia. J Ment Sci 1939; 85: 9991011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9 Scarone, S, Manzone, ML, Gambini, O, Kantzas, I, Limosani, I, D'Agostino, A, et al. The dream as a model for psychosis: an experimental approach using bizarreness as a cognitive marker. Schiz Bull 2008; 34: 515–22.Google Scholar
10 Haffmans, PM, Hoencamp, E, Knegtering, HJ, van Heycop ten Ham, BF. Sleep disturbance in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 1994; 165: 697698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11 Wulff, K, Dijk, D-J, Middleton, B, Foster, RG, Joyce, EM. Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry 2012; 200: 308–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12 Wilson, S, Argyropoulos, S. Sleep in schizophrenia: time for closer attention. Br J Psychiatry 2012; 200: 273–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13 Yung, AR, McGorry, PD. The initial prodrome in psychosis: descriptive and qualitative aspects. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 1996; 30: 587–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14 Ruhrmann, S, Schultze-Lutter, F, Salokangas, RK, Heinimaa, M, Linszen, D, Dingemans, P, et al. Prediction of psychosis in adolescents and young adults at high risk: results from the prospective European prediction of psychosis study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67: 241–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15 Cannon, M, Walsh, E, Hollis, C, et al. Predictors of later schizophrenia and affective psychosis among attendees at a child psychiatry department. Br J Psychiatry 2001; 178: 420–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16 Levin, R, Fireman, G. Nightmare prevalence, nightmare distress, and self-reported psychological disturbance. Sleep 2002; 25: 205–12.Google ScholarPubMed
17 Ohayon, MM, Guilleminault, C, Priest, RG. Night terrors, sleepwalking, and confusional arousals in the general population: their frequency and relationship to other sleep and mental disorders. J Clin Psychiatry 1999; 60: 268–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18 Claridge, G, Clark, K, Davis, C. Nightmares, dreams, and schizotypy. Br J Clin Psychol 1997; 36: 377–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19 Levin, R. Nightmares and schizotypy. Psychiatry 1998; 61: 206–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20 Fisher, H, Lereya, ST, Thompson, A, Lewis, G, Zammit, S, Wolke, D. Childhood parasomnias and psychotic experiences at age 12 in a UK birth cohort. Sleep 2014; 37: 475–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21 Boyd, A, Golding, J, Macleod, J, Lawlor, DA, Fraser, A, Henderson, J, et al. Cohort profile: the ‘children of the 90s’–the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol 2013; 42: 111–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
22 Fraser, A, Macdonald-Wallis, C, Tilling, K, Boyd, A, Golding, J, Davey Smith, G, et al. Cohort profile: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol 2013; 42: 97110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23 Shaffer, D, Fisher, P, Lucas, CP, Dulcan, MK, Schwab-Stone, ME. NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule For Children Version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39: 2838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24 World Health Organization. Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (version 2.0). WHO, 1994.Google Scholar
25 Fisher, HL, Lereya, ST, Thompson, A, Lewis, G, Zammit, S, Wolke, D. Childhood parasomnias and psychotic experiences at age 12 years in a United Kingdom birth cohort. Sleep 2014; 37: 475–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder (4th edn) (DSM-IV). American Psychiatric Association, 1994.Google Scholar
27 Wolke, D. Feeding and sleeping across the lifespan. In Development Through Life: A Handbook for Clinicians (ed Rutter, M and Hay, D): 517–57. Blackwell Scientific Publications, 1994.Google Scholar
28 Wechsler, D. Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd edn) (WISC-III). Psychological Corporation, 1991.Google Scholar
29 Bowen, E, Heron, J, Waylen, A, Wolke, D. Domestic violence risk during and after pregnancy: findings from a British longitudinal study. BJOG 2005; 112: 1083–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30 Goodman, R, Ford, T, Richards, H, Gatward, R, Meltzer, H. The development and well-being assessment: description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000; 41: 645–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31 Angold, A, Costello, EJ, Messer, SC, Pickles, A. Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 1995; 5: 237–49.Google Scholar
32 Angold, A, Erkanli, A, Silberg, J, Eaves, L, Costello, EJ. Depression scale scores in 8-17-year-olds: effects of age and gender. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2002; 43: 1052–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33 Koffel, E, Watson, D. Unusual sleep experiences, dissociation, and schizotypy: evidence for a common domain. Clin Psychol Rev 2009; 29: 548–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34 Laberge, L, Tremblay, RE, Vitaro, F, Montplaisir, J. Development of parasomnias from childhood to early adolescence. Pediatrics 2000; 106: 6774.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35 St James-Roberts, I. The Origins, Prevention and Treatment of Infant Crying and Sleeping Problems. An Evidence-Based Guide for Healthcare Professionals and the Families they Support. Routledge, 2012.Google Scholar
36 Owens, JA, Maxim, R, Nobile, C, McGuinn, M, Msall, M. Parental and self-report of sleep in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2000; 154: 549–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37 Wolke, D, Waylen, A, Samara, M, Steer, C, Goodman, R, Ford, T et al. Selective drop-out in longitudinal studies and non-biased prediction of behaviour disorders. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 249–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38 Mahowald, MW, Schenck, CH. Dissociated states of wakefulness and sleep. Neurology 1992; 42: 4451; discussion 52.Google ScholarPubMed
39 Howland, RH. Sleep-onset rapid eye movement periods in neuropsychiatric disorders: implications for the pathophysiology of psychosis. J Nerv Ment Dis 1997; 185: 730–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40 Rechtschaffen, A, Schulsinger, F, Mednick, SA. Schizophrenia and physiological indices of dreaming. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1964; 10: 8993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41 D'Agostino, A, Limosani, I, Scarone, S. The dreaming brain/mind: a role in understanding complex mental disorders? Front Psychiatry 2012; 3: 3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42 Keshavan, MS, Montrose, DM, Miewald, JM, Jindal, RD. Sleep correlates of cognition in early course psychotic disorders. Schizophr Res 2011; 131: 231–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43 Ferrarelli, F, Peterson, MJ, Sarasso, S, Riedner, BA, Murphy, MJ, Benca, RM, et al. Thalamic dysfunction in schizophrenia suggested by whole-night deficits in slow and fast spindles. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167: 1339–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44 D'Agostino, A, Aletti, G, Carboni, M, Cavallotti, S, Limosani, I, Manzone, M, et al. Are delusional contents replayed during dreams? Conscious Cogn 2013; 22: 708–15.Google ScholarPubMed
45 Schredl, M. Dream research in schizophrenia: methodological issues and a dimensional approach. ConsciousCogn 2011; 20: 1036–41.Google Scholar
46 Gottesmann, C. The dreaming sleep stage: a new neurobiological model of schizophrenia? Neuroscience 2006; 140: 1105–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
47 Gottesmann, C, Gottesman, I. The neurobiological characteristics of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep are candidate endophenotypes of depression, schizophrenia, mental retardation and dementia. Progneurobiol 2007; 81: 237–50.Google ScholarPubMed
48 Lopez, R, Jaussent, I, Scholz, S, Bayard, S, Montplaisir, J, Dauvilliers, Y. Functional impairment in adult sleepwalkers: a case-control study. Sleep 2013; 36: 345–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49 Hartley, S, Barrowclough, C, Haddock, G. Anxiety and depression in psychosis: a systematic review of associations with positive psychotic symptoms. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2013; 128: 327–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50 Terr, LC, Bloch, DA, Michel, BA, Shi, H, Reinhardt, JA, Metayer, S. Children's symptoms in the wake of challenger: a field study of distant-traumatic effects and an outline of related conditions. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156: 1536–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
51 Schreier, A, Wolke, D, Thomas, K, Horwood, J, Hollis, C, Gunnell, D, et al. Prospective study of peer victimization in childhood and psychotic symptoms in a nonclinical population at age 12 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66: 527–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
52 Fisher, HL, Schreier, A, Zammit, S, Maughan, B, Munafò, MR, Lewis, G, et al. Pathways between childhood victimization and psychosis-like symptoms in the ALSPAC birth cohort. Schizophr Bull 2013; 39: 1045–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edn) (DSM-5). American Psychiatric Association, 2013.Google Scholar
54 Neylan, TC, Marmar, CR, Metzler, TJ, Weiss, DS, Zatzick, DF, Delucchi, KL, et al. Sleep disturbances in the Vietnam generation: findings from a nationally representative sample of male Vietnam veterans. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155: 929–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55 Nielsen, T, Levin, R. Nightmares: a new neurocognitive model. Sleep Med Rev 2007; 11: 295310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
56 Hartman, D, Crisp, AH, Sedgwick, P, Borrow, S. Is there a dissociative process in sleepwalking and night terrors? Postgrad Med J 2001; 77: 244–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
57 Arseneault, L, Cannon, M, Fisher, HL, Polanczyk, G, Moffitt, TE, Caspi, A. Childhood trauma and children's emerging psychotic symptoms: a genetically sensitive longitudinal cohort study. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168: 6572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
58 Krakow, B. Nightmare complaints in treatment-seeking patients in clinical sleep medicine settings: diagnostic and treatment implications. Sleep 2006; 29: 1313–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
59 Aurora, RN, Zak, RS, Auerbach, SH, Casey, KR, Chowdhuri, S, Karippot, A, et al. Best practice guide for the treatment of nightmare disorder in adults. J Clin Sleep Med 2010; 6: 389401.Google ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Sleeping problems in childhood and suspected/definite psychotic experiences at 18 years excluding attributions to sleep and fevera

Figure 1

Table 2 Childhood sleeping problems and persistence of suspected/definite psychotic experiences between 12 and 18 years excluding attributions to sleep and fevera

Supplementary material: PDF

Thompson et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Table S1-S5

Download Thompson et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 58.1 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.