Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-24hb2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T18:48:33.583Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Callous–unemotional traits in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Virginia Carter Leno*
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, London
Tony Charman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, London
Andrew Pickles
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, London
Catherine R. G. Jones
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Cardiff
Gillian Baird
Affiliation:
Guy's & St Thomas' NHS Foundation Trust, Newcomen Centre, London
Francesca Happé
Affiliation:
MRC SDGP Centre, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, London
Emily Simonoff
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, King's College London, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience and NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Mental Health, London, UK
*
Virginia Carter Leno, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, 16 De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF. Email: virginia.m.carter_leno@kcl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

People with callous–unemotional traits and also those with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) display sociocognitive difficulties. However, the frequency and neurocognitive correlates of callous–unemotional traits within individuals with ASD are unknown.

Aims

To determine the prevalence of callous–unemotional traits in individuals with ASD and test their association with behavioural and cognitive measures.

Method

Parents of 92 adolescents with ASD completed the Antisocial Processes Screening Device (APSD) for callous–unemotional traits. Adolescents participated in tasks of emotion recognition, theory of mind and cognitive flexibility.

Results

In total 51% (n = 47) scored above a cut-off expected to identify the top 6% on the APSD. Of these 17% (n = 8) had concurrent conduct problems. Regression analyses found callous–unemotional traits were associated with specific impairment in fear recognition but not with theory of mind or cognitive flexibility.

Conclusions

Adolescents with ASD show high rates of callous–unemotional traits but, unlike in the general population, these are not strongly associated with conduct problems. The relationship of callous–unemotional traits to impairments in fear recognition suggests similar affective difficulties as in individuals with callous–unemotional traits without ASD.

Type
Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) licence.
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2015

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterised by a lack of social reciprocity and qualitative impairment in communication, and restricted, repetitive behaviours and interests. 1 Cognitive impairments have been hypothesised to underlie the surface features of behaviour. Individuals with ASD have difficulty recognising emotions Reference Uljarevic and Hamilton2 (although see Jones et al Reference Jones, Pickles, Falcaro, Marsden, Happé and Scott3 ) and understanding the mental states of others, also known as theory of mind, Reference Frith4 both of which are thought to contribute to problems in social functioning. Executive dysfunction is also reported in individuals with ASD, and may underlie restrictive and repetitive symptoms. Impairments occur on tasks of planning, response selection/monitoring and inhibition of pre-potent response in children Reference Happé, Booth, Charlton and Hughes5 and adolescents, Reference Hughes, Russell and Robbins6 as well as reduced cognitive flexibility. Reference Ozonoff and Jensen7 With regard to common coexisting behavioural problems, individuals with ASD are at higher risk of co-occurring psychopathology. Reference Gjevik, Eldevik, Fjæran-Granum and Sponheim8Reference Simonoff, Pickles, Charman, Chandler, Loucas and Baird10 Individuals with ASD display increased antisocial and aggressive behaviour as children Reference Bauminger, Solomon and Rogers11 and oppositional defiant disorder as adolescents. Reference Simonoff, Pickles, Charman, Chandler, Loucas and Baird10 Children presenting with both ASD and oppositional defiant disorder have a more severe pattern of psychiatric symptoms than those without oppositional defiant disorder, with increased symptoms of generalised anxiety, mania, major depressive disorder and perseverative behaviours, in addition to more conduct problems. Reference Gadow, Devincent and Drabick12

Within the heterogeneous presentation of conduct disorder in individuals without ASD, a distinct subgroup with a more severe and stable pattern of antisocial behaviour has been designated as having callous–unemotional traits. Reference Frick and White13 This subgroup has a distinct affective and interpersonal style, including a lack of empathy, an absence of remorse and a tendency to use others for personal gain. Reference Frick and White13 Callous–unemotional traits in youth are a juvenile precursor of, and conceptually similar to, psychopathy. Reference Frick and White13 Underlying cognitive and affective processing difficulties have been proposed to underlie atypical social functioning in individuals with callous–unemotional traits. Adolescents with psychopathic traits Reference Blair, Mitchell, Peschardt, Colledge, Leonard and Shine14 and children with high callous–unemotional trait scores Reference Dadds, Perry, Hawes, Merz, Riddell and Haines15 have difficulties in the recognition of fearful faces. This inability to recognise interpersonal signals of distress has been postulated to underlie atypical empathic development. Reference Blair16 Robust and well-replicated associations between psychopathy and executive dysfunction are also reported, Reference Morgan and Lilienfeld17 including difficulties with cognitive flexibility, attentional switching Reference Newman, Cooke, Forth and Hare18 and response reversal. Reference Mitchell, Colledge, Leonard and Blair19 The explanatory role of attention–deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms in the association between executive functioning and callous–unemotional traits is not well understood; controlling for ADHD has been reported to eradicate the relationship between executive functioning and delinquency in adolescents. Reference Moffitt and Henry20 Individuals with ASD and those with callous–unemotional traits can sometimes present similarly, at least on a superficial level, yet this may be as a result of different aetiological processes. Despite phenotypic similarity, twin studies report largely independent genetic influences (an overlap of 0.43) on autistic and psychopathic traits. Reference Jones, Larsson, Ronald, Rijsdijk, Busfield and Mcmillan21 Comparative studies have suggested that callous–unemotional traits are associated with a selective impairment in affective domains (for example the ability to care about another's feelings), whereas cognitive domains (for example the ability to understand what another is thinking; theory of mind) remain unaffected. Reference Jones, Happe, Gilbert, Burnett and Viding22Reference Lockwood, Bird, Bridge and Viding24 Conversely, individuals with ASD appear to have difficulties in cognitive, but not affective, aspects of interpersonal tasks. Reference Frith4,Reference Jones, Happe, Gilbert, Burnett and Viding22Reference Lockwood, Bird, Bridge and Viding24 Recent neuroimaging studies comparing individuals with callous–unemotional traits and those with ASD have supported the behavioural findings of distinct patterns of theory of mind and affective empathy of the two groups. Reference O'Nions, Sebastian, McCrory, Chantiluke, Happé and Viding25

Very few studies have examined the profile associated with callous–unemotional traits within individuals with ASD. Rogers et al reported that children with ASD and callous–unemotional traits required a longer latency to recognise sad faces, but were not impaired in intelligence, response inhibition or cognitive flexibility. Reference Rogers, Viding, Blair, Frith and Happe26 Comparison of individuals with ASD who were also offenders v. non-offenders showed a specific impairment in fear recognition, yet unimpaired theory of mind and executive functioning in the offending group. Reference Woodbury-Smith, Clare, Holland, Kearns, Staufenberg and Watson27 These studies suggest that some individuals with ASD also show callous–unemotional traits, perhaps as a result of a ‘double-hit’, with the emotional processes implicated in callous–unemotional being affected, in addition to the social cognitive difficulties typically present in individuals with ASD (for example theory of mind impairment). However, whether these findings hold in larger, population-based samples remains unexplored. Individuals with this ‘double-hit’ of ASD and callous–unemotional traits may be a complex yet relatively common presentation within clinics caring for young people with ASD and additional mental health problems, and thus require further study. Previous research on callous–unemotional traits in people with ASD has used selected populations. This paper extends previous findings by addressing the behavioural and cognitive correlates of callous–unemotional traits within a population-based sample of adolescents with ASD. We first investigate the frequency of callous–unemotional traits in adolescents with ASD. We test whether the strong association between these traits and conduct problems reported in the general population also applies to those with ASD, and describe additional psychiatric symptoms associated with callous–unemotional traits. Finally, we test whether a specific impairment in fear recognition is associated with callous–unemotional traits, as in non-ASD populations, and explore whether these traits are associated with difficulties in theory of mind and cognitive functioning.

Method

Participants

A total of 92 adolescents with ASD, who were verbal and had an IQ ≥50 had been assessed on the relevant measures as part of the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP) cohort. This cohort was drawn from 56 946 children living in the South Thames area of the UK and born between July 1990 and December 1991, initially as part of an autism prevalence study (see Baird et al Reference Baird, Simonoff, Pickles, Chandler, Loucas and Meldrum28 for further details). The cohort was assessed at age 12 and 16 years. Assessment at 16 years focused on the cognitive phenotype of ASD and only those who had estimated IQs ≥50 at 12 years were included. Reference Charman, Jones, Pickles, Simonoff, Baird and Happe29 All received a consensus clinical ICD-10 30 ASD diagnoses made using the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) Reference Le Couteur, Rutter, Lord, Rios, Robertson and Holdgrafer31 and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic (ADOS-G) Reference Lord, Risi, Lambrecht, Cook, Leventhal and DiLavore32 at age 12 years. The total number of ICD-10 autism symptoms was recorded. The study was approved by the South East Multicentre Research Ethics Committee (REC) (05/MRE01/67). Written informed consent was obtained from all parents.

Of the participants, 48 met consensus criteria for childhood autism and 44 for other pervasive developmental disorders (ICD-10). There were 84 males and 8 females, and the mean age was 15.5 years (s.d. = 0.5, range 14.7–16.8). Mean Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) Reference Wechsler33 full scale IQ was 84.7 (s.d. = 17.2, range 50–119).

Parental report, when the participants were aged 12, of car ownership and housing tenure were used to construct a crude binary income index (from income differences reported in MacIntryre et al Reference Macintyre, Ellaway, Der, Ford and Hunt34 ) as a family-based measure of deprivation. Neighbourhood deprivation was measured using the Carstairs Index, which combines overcrowding, unemployment, proportion of the population in Registrar General social classes 4 and 5 and households without a car, with total population UK scores ranging from −5.71 (least deprived) to 16.50, median −0.88 (s.d.= 3.41). 35

Measures

Behavioural and psychiatric measures

Callous-unemotional (callous–unemotional) traits. The parent-rated Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD) Reference Frick and Hare36 was completed at 16 years to assess characteristics related to psychopathy. The APSD is a 20-item questionnaire with three underlying dimensions of psychopathic behaviour: impulsiveness, narcissism and callous–unemotional traits. The APSD has good internal consistency, reliability and validity Reference Frick and Hare36 and is predictive of later antisocial outcomes. Reference McMahon, Witkiewitz and Kotler37 The callous–unemotional dimension is made up of six items, scored 0 (not at all true), 1 (sometimes true) or 2 (definitely true): cares about school work, good at keeping promises, feels bad or guilty when does something wrong, concerned about others' feelings, hides feelings from others and keeps the same friends (items are reverse scored where appropriate). For binary classification, adolescents who scored six or above (equal to or above a T-score of 65, comparable to the top 6%) on the callous–unemotional subscale were identified as having callous–unemotional traits present. Reference Frick and Hare36 Internal consistency of the callous–unemotional subscale was moderate in our sample (β = 0.59), similar to that reported elsewhere. Reference Dadds, Fraser, Frost and Hawes38 Item response rates are reported in online supplement DS1.

Behavioural symptoms. The parent-rated Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) Reference Goodman, Ford, Simmons, Gatward and Meltzer39 at 16 years was used to measure psychiatric symptoms. The SDQ comprises three psychiatric subscales of hyperactivity (ADHD symptoms), conduct and emotional problems, along with further subscales of peer-relationship problems and prosocial behaviour. For binary classification, we used the general population-defined cut-off ≥4 on the conduct subscale for ‘definite’ conduct problems. Reference Goodman, Ford, Simmons, Gatward and Meltzer39 Continuous analyses used the conduct, hyperactivity, emotional, peer problems and prosocial subscale scores.

Autism severity. Autism severity was measured by dichotomous diagnostic classification of childhood autism/other pervasive developmental disorder (for details see Baird et al Reference Baird, Simonoff, Pickles, Chandler, Loucas and Meldrum28 ). In addition, clinicians (G.B., T.C. and E.S.) used the diagnostic information from the ADI-R and ADOS-G to score the 12 ICD-10 symptoms that comprise the ASD diagnoses (score range 0–12, with a higher score indicating greater severity). The parent-rated Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) Reference Constantino, Davis, Todd, Schindler, Gross and Brophy40 measured the severity of social difficulties associated with ASD. The SRS comprises 65 items providing a total score of autistic traits. This was scored at 12 years in 60 participants and at 16 years in 27, where data were missing at 12 years.

Neurocognitive measures

Fuller details of the neurocognitive tasks are given in online supplement DS1. All were administered at 16 years.

Cognitive ability. We measured IQ with the WASI Reference Wechsler33 generating full-scale, verbal and performance IQ measures.

Emotion recognition. The Ekman–Friesen test of affect recognition was used. Reference Ekman, Friesen and Press41 The total number of correct responses for each of the six emotions (happy, sad, fear, surprise, anger, disgust), alongside the total overall score served as the measure of emotion recognition ability. Data from this task have previously been reported by SNAP. Reference Jones, Pickles, Falcaro, Marsden, Happé and Scott3

Theory of mind. The Strange Stories task Reference Happé42 was used as a general measure of mental state understanding. Participants were read a series of stories, which were also available in front of them and accompanied by an appropriate illustration. At the end of each story, they were asked a question about the text. The outcome variable was the average score across the four theory of mind stories (score range 0–2, with a higher score indicating better performance).

The Frith–Happé animations Reference Abell, Happé and Frith43 consist of a series of silent videos of two-dimensional animations, requiring the participant to understand intentionality behind the moving shapes. Four animations depicted theory of mind interactions and two goal-directed interactions. The outcome variable was the average intentionality score for the four theory of mind items (score range 0–5). Data from this task have previously been reported by SNAP. Reference Jones, Swettenham, Charman, Marsden, Tregay and Baird44

The Penny Hiding task Reference Baron Cohen45 was used as a naturalistic and non-verbal measure, specifically indexing the participant's ability to deceive the experimenter. The participant was given six trials of hiding the penny. Responses are coded for the type of deception errors made, with a total score calculated. It was possible to display more than one error on a trial (score range 0–30).

The Combined False Belief task (designed by Rhonda Booth, Institute of Psychiatry, see online supplement DS1 for details of the task) is a combination of first- and second-order false belief tasks based on previous tasks measuring false belief understanding. Reference Baron Cohen46,Reference Bowler47 The two outcome variables were performance on the first- and second-order parts of the story. If the participant failed the false belief question then the overall score was automatically set to zero (score range 0–2 for the first order, 0–3 for the second order false belief task).

The Second-Order False Belief task designed by Bowler was also included, which has greater verbal demands than the combined false belief task. Reference Bowler47 The outcome variable is whether the participant is able to understand the second-order false belief. Participants were awarded one point for passing the false belief question, and a further point for passing the justification, reality and memory questions; participants failing one or more of these questions were awarded a score of 0 (score range 0–2).

Executive functioning. The Card Sort task was used as a measure of cognitive flexibility and response reversal. Reference Tregay, Gilmour and Charman48 Participants had to correctly sort cards to one of three alternative sets across three trials, with the correct set varying in each trial. The key variable was the number of sorts required to reach criterion. In the present analyses, we included only those participants who demonstrated an understanding of the rule in the first trial by reaching criterion before the end. The number of sorts required in the second and third trials was divided into four levels: top half (scores 12–18, n = 40), third quartile (scores 19–24, n = 21), bottom quartile (scores 25–40, n = 17) and those who did not reach criterion by the end of both trials (n = 7).

The Trail Making task was included as a measure of attentional switching and response reversal. Reference Reitan and Wolfson49 Participants were asked to ‘join the dots’ in numerical order, then, in a second trial, in alphabetical order, followed by a third trial switching between numbers and letters. The difference between the time taken on the first and the third trial comprised a measure of switching ability. Because the data were highly skewed, a square-root transformed score was used in the present analyses. Data from this task have previously been reported by SNAP. Reference Simonoff, Jones, Pickles, Happé, Baird and Charman50

Statistical analysis

All data reduction and statistical analysis were undertaken in Stata version 11. Descriptive statistics are tabulated by group; however, as the group above cut-off for both callous–unemotional traits and conduct problems was small, associations with callous–unemotional traits were explored using the continuous measure. In analysing the relationship among behavioural measures, because of well-recognised cross-domain correlations, any significant bivariate associations were subsequently covaried for conduct problems, in order to identify independent relationships between callous–unemotional traits and behavioural measures. Associations were also covaried for autism severity, to identify associations with callous–unemotional traits above those accounted for by autism severity. The ICD-10 measure was used as a covariate, as it provides an independent measure in addition to parent-rated measures of behaviour. Finally, associations between callous–unemotional traits and performance on the Card Sort and Trail Making tasks were covaried for hyperactivity because of previously reported relationships in other samples. In general, dependent variables were continuous. However, ordinal logistic regression was used for the four-level Card Sort scale. Both the emotion recognition task, in which six different emotions were assessed, and the theory of mind tasks were analysed using multivariate regression to increase efficiency and reduce type 1 errors from multiple testing. For both analyses, the overall combined emotion recognition/theory of mind score was first entered into the regression and subsequently each emotion/theory of mind task was entered as a separate predictor in the regression model. Significance of effects was determined from Wald tests using the robust form of the parameter covariance matrix.

Results

Of the 92 adolescents with ASD, 51% (95% CI 40–61%, n = 47) scored above the designated cut-off for callous–unemotional traits, expected to identify the top 6% of callous–unemotional scores in the general population (equivalent to a T-score of 65, according to the APSD manual Reference Frick and Hare36 ). Within the high callous–unemotional group (less one participant who was missing SDQ data), only 17% (8/46) also scored above the conduct problems threshold, in contrast to 9% (4/45) with low callous–unemotional scores. These rates of conduct problems were not significantly different (P = 0.19, Fisher's exact test).

Table 1 gives the descriptive characteristics for three groups; those below callous–unemotional (CU) and conduct problems (CP) threshold, (CU− CP− group, n = 41), those above callous–unemotional but not conduct problems threshold (CU+ CP− group, n = 38) and those above threshold for both callous–unemotional and conduct problems (CU+ CP+, n = 8). The group with low callous–unemotional and high conduct problems scores was too small to include (n = 4). There were no significant differences in age, gender ratio, type of ASD diagnosis or ICD-10 symptom severity among these three groups. The levels of family and neighbourhood disadvantage were also similar. However, consistent with the literature on conduct problems in typically developing adolescents, Reference Moffitt and Silva51 the CU+ CP+ group had a significantly lower full-scale and verbal, but not performance IQ.

TABLE 1 Sample characteristics and behavioural symptoms according to the presence of callous–unemotional traits, as rated by the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD) and conduct problems, as rated by the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)

CU− CP−
(n = 41)
CU+ CP−
(n = 38)
CU+ CP+
(n = 8)
Between-group comparisons
Male, n (%) 37 (90.2) 35 (92.1) 7 (87.5) ns
Childhood autism (v. other PDD), n (%) 20 (48.8) 21 (55.3) 5 (62.5) ns
ICD-10 symptom severity, a mean (s.d.) 7.78 (2.24) 8.26 (2.64) 9.38 (2.39) ns
With family disadvantage, b n (%) 5 (15.6) 7 (24.1) 1 (16.7) ns
Neighbourhood deprivation, c mean (s.d.) −1.09 (2.20) −0.58 (2.60) −0.15 (0.81) ns
WASI, mean (s.d.)
    Full-scale IQ 85.90 (18.08) 85.45 (17.00) 72.00 (12.07) CU+ CP+ <CU+ CP−, CU− CP−*
    Verbal IQ 81.37 (18.05) 83.00 (17.44) 67.50 (10.84) CU+ CP+ <CU+ CP−, CU− CP−*
    Performance IQ 93.20 (19.37) 90.00 (17.55) 81.25 (13.84) ns
APSD, mean (s.d.)
    Total 10.59 (4.28) 14.55 (4.27) 21.75 (6.18) CU+ CP+ >CU+ CP− >CU− CP−**
    Callous-unemotional 3.63 (1.36) 7.47 (1.29) 8.00 (1.31) CU+ CP+, CU+ CP− >CU− CP−**
    Impulsivity 4.10 (1.77) 4.26 (1.90) 7.13 (2.42) CU+ CP+, CU+ CP− >CU− CP−**
    Narcissism 2.59 (2.26) 2.58 (2.25) 5.50 (3.16) CU+ CP+, CU+ CP− >CU− CP−**
SQD, mean (s.d.)
    Conduct problems 1.32 (1.04) 1.37 (1.02) 5.25 (1.58) CU+ CP+ >CU+ CP−, CU− CP−**
    Hyperactivity 5.56 (2.76) 5.79 (2.32) 7.25 (2.05) ns
    Emotional symptoms 3.63 (2.55) 3.34 (2.33) 3.75 (2.31) ns
    Peer problems 4.98 (2.38) 5.92 (2.42) 6.25 (2.96) ns
    Prosocial behaviour d 6.15 (1.93) 4.22 (2.02) 4.13 (1.89) CU+ CP+, CU+ CP− <CU− CP−**
SRS total, e mean (s.d.) 83.00 (21.67) 99.57 (20.52) 113.38 (21.00) CU+ CP+, CU+ CP− >CU− CP−**

APSD, Antisocial Process Screening Device; CP, conduct problems; CU, callous-unemotional traits; ns, non-significant; PDD, pervasive developmental disorder; WASI, Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; SDQ Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; SRS Social Responsiveness Scale;

a. CU− CP−, n = 37; CU+ CP−, n = 34.

b. CU− CP−, n = 32; CU+ CP−, n = 29; CU+ CP+, n = 6.

c. CU− CP−, n = 40.

d. CU+ CP−, n = 37.

e. CU− CP−, n = 39; CU+ CP−, n = 37.

* P<0.05

** P<0.01.

Behavioural symptoms

Regressions assessing the association between callous–unemotional traits and behavioural symptoms are shown in Table 2. SDQ subscales of conduct problems, hyperactivity and peer-relationship problems were all significantly and positively associated with callous–unemotional traits, whereas prosocial behaviour was negatively related. When conduct problems were accounted for, the associations with peer-relationship problems and prosocial behaviour remained significant but hyperactivity was no longer associated with callous–unemotional traits. Emotional symptoms were unrelated to callous–unemotional traits. The SRS was strongly and significantly associated with callous–unemotional traits and this relationship was unaffected by covarying for conduct problems. When associations were covaried for both conduct problems and autism severity, associations between peer problems and prosocial behaviour and callous–unemotional traits remained significant. Correlations between callous–unemotional traits and psychiatric and autistic symptoms are displayed in online Table DS1.

TABLE 2 Psychiatric and autistic measure associations with callous–unemotional traits as rated by the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD) (standardised) a

Unadjusted Adjusted for conduct problems Adjusted for conduct problems
and autism severity
Emotional and behavioural problems
at 16 (parent-rated)
β P β P β P
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire
    Conduct problems 0.17 0.02 NA
    Hyperactivity 0.24 0.03 0.18 0.11 0.11 0.38
    Emotional symptoms −0.02 0.82 NA - NA -
    Peer relations 0.30 <0.01 0.28 0.01 0.28 0.03
    Prosocial behaviour −0.50 <0.001 −0.51 <0.001 −0.50 <0.001
    Social responsiveness scale, total 4.78 <0.001 4.70 <0.001 NA -

a. Adjusted associations first account for the presence of conduct problems, and second for the presence of conduct problems and level of autism severity as rated by ICD-10 symptom severity.

Neurocognitive differences among groups

Total emotional recognition and theory of mind task scores by group are shown in Table 3, together with regressions assessing the relationship between callous–unemotional traits and task performance. In line with findings from typically developing populations, there was no overall association between emotion recognition and callous–unemotional traits (P = 0.21). As predicted, we identified a specific relationship between higher callous–unemotional traits and poorer recognition of fear (β = −0.30, P = 0.02). As we hypothesised a priori that fear recognition would be selectively affected, no corrections for multiple comparisons were performed. When full-scale IQ was controlled for, the relationship between callous–unemotional traits and fear recognition remained significant (β = −0.24, P = 0.04). No overall association was found between callous–unemotional traits and either overall theory of mind (P = 0.73) or with any individual theory of mind measure.

TABLE 3 Neurocognitive correlates of callous–unemotional traits as rated by the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD): scores and multivariate regression of callous-unemotional traits on emotional recognition and theory of mind tasks (standardised)

Group, mean (s.d.) Unadjusted test statistic Group, n
CU− CP− CU+ CP− CU+ CP+ F (d.f.) β P CU− CP− CU+ CP− CU+ CP+
Emotion recognition
Overall test of effect 1.44 (6,87) 0.21 40 36 8
Specific emotion 40 36 8
    Happiness 9.88 (0.40) 9.94 (0.23) 9.38 (1.41) −0.01 0.71
    Sadness 7.55 (2.37) 7.14 (1.94) 7.13 (0.64) −0.15 0.13
    Fear 6.35 (2.65) 5.50 (2.85) 5.38 (2.00) −0.30 0.02
    Anger 7.18 (1.93) 6.75 (1.87) 7.50 (1.93) −0.03 0.76
    Surprise 8.30 (2.57) 8.50 (1.98) 7.50 (2.73) −0.11 0.32
    Disgust 4.85 (3.04) 4.53 (2.97) 3.63 (1.77) −0.20 0.12
Theory of mind
Overall test of effect 0.63 (7,76) 0.73 41 38 8
Specific task
    Strange Stories 0.76 (0.49) 0.98 (0.54) 0.50 (0.32) 0.01 0.86 37 34 7
    Penny Hiding Errors 1.66 (0.73) 1.84 (0.82) 1.75 (0.89) 0.02 0.64 41 38 8
    Frith-Happé animations 2.91 (1.07) 2.86 (0.87) 2.67 (0.86) −0.03 0.48 38 34 6
    Combined First Order False Belief 1.71 (0.68) 1.68 (0.71) 1.50 (0.93) −0.00 0.86 41 37 8
    Combined Second Order False Belief 1.71 (1.12) 1.59 (1.19) 1.63 (1.19) −0.03 0.53 41 37 8
    Second Order False Belief 1.61 (0.93) 1.67 (0.78) 1.57 (0.98) −0.03 0.44 36 33 7

CP, conduct problems; CU, callous–unemotional traits.

There was no association between callous–unemotional traits and cognitive flexibility as measured using the Card Sort (odds ratio (OR) = 1.06, P = 0.44) or difference scores on the Trail Making task (β = 0.11, P = 0.30). Controlling for hyperactivity left the pattern of results unchanged. Total scores by group are displayed in Table 4. Correlations between callous–unemotional traits and all neurocognitive measures are displayed in online Tables DS2 and DS3.

TABLE 4 Neurocognitive correlates of callous–unemotional traits as rated by the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD): scores and association with callous–unemotional traits on Trail Making and Card Sort tasks (standardised)

CU− CP− group
(n = 41)
CU+ CP− group
(n = 36)
CU+ CP+ group
(n = 8)
β Odds ratio P
Trail making difference score, mean (s.d.) 7.22 (2.23) 7.21 (2.63) 7.21 (1.84) 0.11 0.30
Card sort trials to criterion, n 1.06 0.44
    Top 50% 20 16 4
    51–75% 9 12 0
    76%+ 7 6 4
    Did not meet criterion 5 2 0

CP, Conduct problems; CU, Callous–unemotional traits.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first report of the prevalence, psychiatric correlates and neurocognitive profile of callous–unemotional traits within adolescents with ASD using a population-based sample. We found that 51% scored in the top 6% for callous–unemotional traits, reflecting nearly a tenfold increase over the general population. Reference Fontaine, McCrory, Boivin, Moffitt and Viding52,Reference Collishaw, Maughan, Goodman and Pickles53 Our prevalence rate is higher than that reported in a previous study of callous–unemotional traits in a smaller, selected sample of young people with ASD, which found 36% were ‘high’ in callous–unemotional traits. Reference Rogers, Viding, Blair, Frith and Happe26 The previous study used teacher-reported callous–unemotional traits and ours parent-report; informant type could be related to these differences. Both previous work and our results suggest an increase in callous–unemotional traits in individuals with ASD, compared with the general population. We also demonstrated that callous–unemotional traits in adolescents with ASD are associated with the same deficit in fear recognition previously reported in typically developing samples.

Causes of the high prevalence of callous–unemotional traits in ASD

There are two possible explanations for the remarkably high prevalence of callous–unemotional traits in our sample of adolescents with ASD. First, people with ASD may be at increased risk of developing callous–unemotional traits. It may be that the cognitive impairments associated with ASD increase the risk of developing these traits. Second, the overlap could be as a result of superficial similarity in behaviours, assessed on measures such as the APSD, which may be in part tapping ASD characteristics, leading to an artificial inflation of estimates of callous–unemotional traits. Questionnaires measuring characteristics such as empathy may fail to distinguish affective and cognitive components, which some argue are distinct traits, and are differentially associated with ASD and callous–unemotional traits. Reference Jones, Happe, Gilbert, Burnett and Viding22Reference Lockwood, Bird, Bridge and Viding24 As some behavioural characteristics associated with callous–unemotional traits can be superficially similar to features of ASD (such as lack of sensitivity to the feelings of others), instruments designed to measure callous–unemotional traits in typical populations may not be sensitive enough to discriminate between overlapping aspects of ASD and callous–unemotional traits (for example being able to identify v. caring about emotions of others). However, as we found the specific deficit in fear recognition difficulty associated with callous–unemotional traits in non-ASD populations, we suggest that the APSD may be sensitive enough to distinguish a meaningfully different subgroup of individuals with ASD with additional callous–unemotional traits. It would be interesting in future work to examine alexithymia (impaired ability to reflect on and report own emotions) in this subgroup, given recent evidence that emotion processing and empathy deficits are a function of co-occurring alexithymia rather than characteristics of ASD itself. Reference Bird and Cook54

Within our total sample of adolescents with ASD, 13% (12/92) scored above a cut-off indicative of conduct problems. This rate is similar to those reported in samples of typically developing adolescents. Reference Moffitt and Silva51 Of the group classified as above cut-off for callous–unemotional traits, only 17% displayed concurrent conduct problems. This contrasts with general population findings; in a community sample of 7- to 12-year-olds, those with callous–unemotional traits showed a 0.95 probability of having concomitant conduct problems, and only 0.2% of those with these traits scored below cut-off for conduct problems. Reference Fontaine, McCrory, Boivin, Moffitt and Viding52 The dissociation of callous–unemotional from conduct problems in ASD may indicate a different cognitive underpinning. Alternatively, the sociocognitive difficulties associated with ASD may lead to reduced exposure to peers and prevent individuals from engaging in the more socialised and peer-related aspects of conduct-disordered behaviour.

Behavioural correlates of callous–unemotional traits in ASD

Callous–unemotional traits were associated with an increase in conduct problems, hyperactivity and peer problems, and a decrease in prosocial behaviour. However, when conduct problems were accounted for, the relationship between callous–unemotional traits and hyperactivity became non-significant. Given the conceptualisation of callous–unemotional traits as being in part because of an inability to emotionally resonate with others, our finding of increased callous–unemotional traits being associated with difficulties in interpersonal functioning (as indexed by increased peer problems and a decrease in prosocial behaviour) was expected, and is in line with previous research. Reference Frick and White13,Reference Moffitt and Silva51 Our results therefore provide some evidence for similar behavioural profiles associated with callous–unemotional traits in individuals with ASD, as in those without ASD.

Surface-level similarities in callous–unemotional traits and ASD with respect to impairments in interpersonal functioning may lead to certain behavioural problems being misinterpreted as being solely because of ASD, whereas callous–unemotional traits may also be driving this behaviour. Understanding of the behavioural correlates of these traits in ASD may be vital to unpicking the heterogeneous presentation of ASD. Although the association of callous–unemotional traits with prosocial behaviour and peer problems could be simply manifestations of more severe ASD, we suggest this is not the case. When both conduct problems and autism severity were accounted for, the relationships between callous–unemotional traits and both prosocial behaviour and peer problems remained, suggesting these traits selectively have an impact on the presentation of ASD beyond difficulties driven by autism severity.

Neurocognitive profile of callous–unemotional traits in ASD

The present findings partially support the idea that the cognitive correlates of callous–unemotional traits in people with ASD are similar to those in the typically developing population, suggesting a shared underlying aetiology. Those with callous–unemotional traits and concurrent conduct problems had significantly lower verbal and full-scale IQ than both those with callous–unemotional traits alone and those scoring below threshold for both callous–unemotional traits and conduct problems. This association is in line with previous research. Reference Moffitt and Silva51 No differences were found between individuals scoring below the callous–unemotional and conduct problems threshold, or above the threshold for callous–unemotional traits without conduct problems, suggesting the IQ relationship is with conduct problems.

Previous literature also reports difficulties in the recognition of fearful and sad emotions in children and adolescents with psychopathic and callous–unemotional traits Reference Blair, Mitchell, Peschardt, Colledge, Leonard and Shine14,Reference Dadds, Perry, Hawes, Merz, Riddell and Haines15 and individuals with ASD with a history of offending. Reference Woodbury-Smith, Clare, Holland, Kearns, Staufenberg and Watson27 In line with our predictions, our findings of a similar selective association between callous–unemotional traits and fear recognition in adolescents with ASD supports the idea of comparable disturbance in affective domains, and subsequently similar aetiology of callous–unemotional traits in individuals with and without ASD. Unlike previous studies of callous–unemotional traits in individuals with ASD, Reference Woodbury-Smith, Clare, Holland, Kearns, Staufenberg and Watson27 we did not find impairment in the recognition of sad faces; this may have been because of decreased power within our sample. We found no differences on any of the theory of mind tasks, suggesting no additional impairment in cognitive domains associated with callous–unemotional traits among individuals with ASD, similar to previous findings. Reference Jones, Happe, Gilbert, Burnett and Viding22,Reference Schwenck, Mergenthaler, Keller, Zech, Salehi and Taurines23 The lack of association between callous–unemotional traits and theory of mind is unlikely to be because of theory of mind task insensitivity, as all tasks aside from the Second Order False Belief task showed a significant relationship with at least one measure of autism severity (online Table DS4). Our findings support a selective affective difficulty but no sociocognitive difficulties, similar to that reported in individuals with callous–unemotional traits without ASD. Reference Jones, Happe, Gilbert, Burnett and Viding22Reference Lockwood, Bird, Bridge and Viding24

We found no association between callous–unemotional traits and cognitive flexibility, consistent with previous findings in ASD Reference Jones, Happe, Gilbert, Burnett and Viding22,Reference Schwenck, Mergenthaler, Keller, Zech, Salehi and Taurines23 but different from those in the general population. Reference Newman, Cooke, Forth and Hare18 Previous studies suggest associations between callous–unemotional traits and measures of executive functioning may be driven by comorbid ADHD, Reference Moffitt and Henry20 thus we accounted for hyperactivity within our analyses. This did not change our results, suggesting callous–unemotional traits may indeed present differently with respect to cognitive flexibility when found in individuals with ASD, however these findings require replication.

Strengths and limitations

Strengths of this study include use of a population-based sample of adolescents with carefully characterised ASD. A wide range of psychiatric and cognitive characteristics were assessed, including many that have been previously related to callous–unemotional traits in the typically developing population. This allowed us to explore a range of variables involved in the presentation of callous–unemotional traits, and the sample used suggests any conclusions drawn regarding prevalence rates of callous–unemotional traits in ASD and their psychiatric/cognitive correlates are likely to be representative of the adolescent ASD population.

Limitations of the present study include the use of mainly parent-reported questionnaires of behavioural symptoms, including callous–unemotional traits; however the validity of self-reported behavioural measures in the ASD population still needs to be established. Behavioural items indexing callous–unemotional traits on the APSD may overlap with ASD symptoms, thus more sensitive measures of these traits in ASD require development. Second, we did not have sufficient statistical power to include a comparison group of typically developing adolescents with which to compare our findings. Ideally, future work would examine callous–unemotional traits in typically developing samples compared with individuals with ASD matched for intellectual level.

Implications

Clinicians have been concerned that some people with ASD appear to show high levels of callous–unemotional traits and the present study supports this clinical impression for a subset of individuals. However, callous–unemotional traits in individuals with ASD are less strongly associated with conduct problems than is the case in individuals without ASD. Therefore clinicians should be sensitive to the differential behavioural correlates associated with callous–unemotional traits in individuals with ASD, as compared with other clinical groups. Parents, caregivers and other people working with individuals with ASD may require specific psychoeducation about these traits, which are often distressing to others but are not necessarily a predictor of antisocial behaviour.

The present findings partially support a shared aetiology of callous–unemotional traits in people with ASD and the general population as indexed by impairments in affective domains, but also suggest differences, particularly in the realm of cognitive flexibility. Further study of callous–unemotional traits from both a behavioural and cognitive perspective in individuals with ASD with and without conduct problems would clarify the role of these traits in predisposing individuals to conduct problems and antisocial behaviour.

Funding

The study was funded by the Medical Research Council (G0400065). The authors acknowledge financial support from the Department of Health via the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre and Dementia Unit awarded to South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust in partnership with King's College London and King's College Hospital NHS Foundation Trust. This paper presents independent research funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR). The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

A.P. receives royalties from the Social Communication Questionnaire and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic instruments. F.H. received a one-off consultancy payment from Novartis ($3000) in March 2011.

References

1 American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edn) (DSM-5). APA, 2013.Google Scholar
2 Uljarevic, M, Hamilton, A. Recognition of emotions in autism: a formal meta-analysis. J Autism Dev Disord 2012; 43: 1517–26.Google Scholar
3 Jones, CR, Pickles, A, Falcaro, M, Marsden, AJ, Happé, F, Scott, SK, et al. A multimodal approach to emotion recognition ability in autism spectrum disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2011; 52: 275–85.Google Scholar
4 Frith, U. Mind blindness and the brain in autism. Neuron 2001; 32: 969–79.Google Scholar
5 Happé, F, Booth, R, Charlton, R, Hughes, C. Executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: examining profiles across domains and ages. Brain Cogn 2006; 61: 2539.Google Scholar
6 Hughes, C, Russell, J, Robbins, TW. Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia 1994; 32: 477–92.Google Scholar
7 Ozonoff, S, Jensen, J. Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 1999; 29: 171–7.Google Scholar
8 Gjevik, E, Eldevik, S, Fjæran-Granum, T, Sponheim, E. Kiddie-SADS reveals high rates of DSM-IV disorders in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 2011; 41: 761–9.Google Scholar
9 Leyfer, OT, Folstein, SE, Bacalman, S, Davis, NO, Dinh, E, Morgan, J, et al. Comorbid psychiatric disorders in children with autism: interview development and rates of disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 2006; 36: 849–61.Google Scholar
10 Simonoff, E, Pickles, A, Charman, T, Chandler, S, Loucas, T, Baird, G. Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2008; 47: 921–9.Google Scholar
11 Bauminger, N, Solomon, M, Rogers, SJ. Externalizing and internalizing behaviors in ASD. Autism Res 2010; 3: 101–12.Google Scholar
12 Gadow, KD, Devincent, CJ, Drabick, DA. Oppositional defiant disorder as a clinical phenotype in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 2008; 38: 1302–10.Google Scholar
13 Frick, PJ, White, SF. Research review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008; 49: 359–75.Google Scholar
14 Blair, RJR, Mitchell, D, Peschardt, K, Colledge, E, Leonard, R, Shine, J, et al. Reduced sensitivity to others' fearful expressions in psychopathic individuals. Pers Individ Dif 2004; 37: 1111–22.Google Scholar
15 Dadds, MR, Perry, Y, Hawes, DJ, Merz, S, Riddell, AC, Haines, DJ, et al. Attention to the eyes and fear-recognition deficits in child psychopathy. Br J Psychiatry 2006; 189: 280–1.Google Scholar
16 Blair, RJR. Applying a cognitive neuroscience perspective to the disorder of psychopathy. Dev Psychopathol 2005; 17: 865–91.Google Scholar
17 Morgan, AB, Lilienfeld, SO. A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clin Psychol Rev 2000; 20: 113–36.Google Scholar
18 Newman, JP. Psychopathic behavior: an information processing perspective. In Psychopathy: Theory, Research and Implications for Society (eds Cooke, DJ, Forth, AE, Hare, RD): 81104. Springer, 1998.Google Scholar
19 Mitchell, D, Colledge, E, Leonard, A, Blair, RJR. Risky decisions and response reversal: is there evidence of orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in psychopathic individuals? Neuropsychologia 2002; 40: 2013–22.Google Scholar
20 Moffitt, TE, Henry, B. Neuropsychological assessment of executive functions in self-reported delinquents. Dev Psychopathol 1989; 1: 105–18.Google Scholar
21 Jones, A, Larsson, H, Ronald, A, Rijsdijk, F, Busfield, P, Mcmillan, A, et al. Phenotypic and aetiological associations between psychopathic tendencies, autistic traits, and emotion attribution. Crim Just Behav 2009; 36: 1198–212.Google Scholar
22 Jones, A, Happe, F, Gilbert, F, Burnett, S, Viding, E. Feeling, caring, knowing: different types of empathy deficit in boys with psychopathic tendencies and autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2010; 51: 1188–97.Google Scholar
23 Schwenck, C, Mergenthaler, J, Keller, K, Zech, J, Salehi, S, Taurines, R, et al. Empathy in children with autism and conduct disorder: group-specific profiles and developmental aspects. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2012; 53: 651–9.Google Scholar
24 Lockwood, PL, Bird, G, Bridge, M, Viding, E. Dissecting empathy: high levels of psychopathic and autistic traits are characterised by difficulties in different social information processing domains. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7: 760.Google Scholar
25 O'Nions, E, Sebastian, C, McCrory, E, Chantiluke, K, Happé, F, Viding, E. Neural bases of theory of mind in children with autism spectrum disorders and children with conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. Dev Sci 2014; 17: 786–96.Google Scholar
26 Rogers, J, Viding, E, Blair, RJ, Frith, U, Happe, F. Autism spectrum disorder and psychopathy: shared cognitive underpinnings or double hit? Psychol Med 2006; 36: 1789–98.Google Scholar
27 Woodbury-Smith, M, Clare, I, Holland, A, Kearns, A, Staufenberg, E, Watson, P. A case-control study of offenders with high functioning autistic spectrum disorders. J Forens Psychiatry Psychol 2005; 16: 747–63.Google Scholar
28 Baird, G, Simonoff, E, Pickles, A, Chandler, S, Loucas, T, Meldrum, D, et al. Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Lancet 2006; 368: 210–5.Google Scholar
29 Charman, T, Jones, CRG, Pickles, A, Simonoff, E, Baird, G, Happe, F. Defining the cognitive phenotype of autism. Brain Res 2011; 1380: 1021.Google Scholar
30 World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. WHO, 1992.Google Scholar
31 Le Couteur, A, Rutter, M, Lord, C, Rios, P, Robertson, S, Holdgrafer, M, et al. Autism Diagnostic Interview: a standardized investigator-based instrument. J Autism Dev Disord 1989; 19: 363–87.Google Scholar
32 Lord, C, Risi, S, Lambrecht, L, Cook, EH, Leventhal, BL, DiLavore, PC, et al. The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2000; 30: 205–23.Google Scholar
33 Wechsler, D. The Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence – UK. The Psychological Corporation, 1999.Google Scholar
34 Macintyre, S, Ellaway, A, Der, G, Ford, G, Hunt, K. Do housing tenure and car access predict health because they are simply markers of income or self esteem? A Scottish study. J Epidemiol Community Health 1998; 52: 657–64.Google Scholar
35 National-Statistics. Measuring deprivation in England and Wales using 2001 Carstairs scores. Health Stat Q 2006; 31: 2833.Google Scholar
36 Frick, PJ, Hare, RD. Antisocial Process Screening Device: APSD. Multi-Health Systems, 2001.Google Scholar
37 McMahon, RJ, Witkiewitz, K, Kotler, JS. Predictive validity of callous–unemotional traits measured in early adolescence with respect to multiple antisocial outcomes. J Abnorm Psychol 2010; 119: 752.Google Scholar
38 Dadds, MR, Fraser, J, Frost, A, Hawes, DJ. Disentangling the underlying dimensions of psychopathy and conduct problems in childhood: a community study. J Consult Clin Psychol 2005; 73: 400.Google Scholar
39 Goodman, R, Ford, T, Simmons, H, Gatward, R, Meltzer, H. Using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) to screen for child psychiatric disorders in a community sample. Br J Psychiatry 2000; 177: 534–9.Google Scholar
40 Constantino, JN, Davis, SA, Todd, RD, Schindler, MK, Gross, MM, Brophy, SL, et al. Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the autism diagnostic interview-revised. J Autism Dev Disord 2003; 33: 427–33.Google Scholar
41 Ekman, P, Friesen, WV, Press, CP. Pictures of Facial Affect. Consulting Psychologists Press, 1975.Google Scholar
42 Happé, F. An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters' thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. J Autism Dev Disord 1994; 24: 129–54.Google Scholar
43 Abell, F, Happé, F, Frith, U. Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cog Dev 2000; 15: 116.Google Scholar
44 Jones, CR, Swettenham, J, Charman, T, Marsden, AJ, Tregay, J, Baird, G, et al. No evidence for a fundamental visual motion processing deficit in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res 2011; 4: 347–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45 Baron Cohen, S. Out of sight or out of mind? Another look at deception in autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1992; 33: 1141–55.Google Scholar
46 Baron Cohen, S. The autistic child's theory of mind: a case of specific developmental delay. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1989; 30: 285–97.Google Scholar
47 Bowler, DM. “Theory of Mind” in Asperger's syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1992; 33: 877–93.Google Scholar
48 Tregay, J, Gilmour, J, Charman, T. Childhood rituals and executive functions. Br J Dev Psychol 2009; 27: 283–96.Google Scholar
49 Reitan, RM, Wolfson, D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation. Neuropsychology Press, 1985.Google Scholar
50 Simonoff, E, Jones, CR, Pickles, A, Happé, F, Baird, G, Charman, T. Severe mood problems in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2012; 53: 1157–66.Google Scholar
51 Moffitt, TE, Silva, PA. IQ and delinquency: a direct test of the differential detection hypothesis. J Abnorm Psychol 1988; 97: 330.Google Scholar
52 Fontaine, NM, McCrory, EJ, Boivin, M, Moffitt, TE, Viding, E. Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous–unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. J Abnorm Psychol 2011; 120: 730.Google Scholar
53 Collishaw, S, Maughan, B, Goodman, R, Pickles, A. Time trends in adolescent mental health. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004; 45: 1350–62.Google Scholar
54 Bird, G, Cook, R. Mixed emotions: The contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Transl Psychiatry 2013; 3: e285.Google Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Sample characteristics and behavioural symptoms according to the presence of callous–unemotional traits, as rated by the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD) and conduct problems, as rated by the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)

Figure 1

TABLE 2 Psychiatric and autistic measure associations with callous–unemotional traits as rated by the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD) (standardised)a

Figure 2

TABLE 3 Neurocognitive correlates of callous–unemotional traits as rated by the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD): scores and multivariate regression of callous-unemotional traits on emotional recognition and theory of mind tasks (standardised)

Figure 3

TABLE 4 Neurocognitive correlates of callous–unemotional traits as rated by the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD): scores and association with callous–unemotional traits on Trail Making and Card Sort tasks (standardised)

Supplementary material: PDF

Leno et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Material

Download Leno et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 202 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.