Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ws8qp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T13:53:28.263Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Visual cortical excitability in dementia with Lewy bodies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

John-Paul Taylor*
Affiliation:
Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne
Michael Firbank
Affiliation:
Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne
John T. O'Brien
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge Biomedical Campus, Cambridge, UK
*
John-Paul Taylor, Institute of Neuroscience, Henry Wellcome Building for Neurology, Newcastle University, Framlington Place, Newcastle upon Tyne NE2 4HH, UK. Email: john-paul.taylor@ncl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Alterations in the visual system may underlie visual hallucinations in dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB). However, cortical excitability as measured by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) activation of lower visual areas (V1–3) to visual stimuli appear normal in DLB. We explored the relationship between TMS-determined phosphene threshold and fMRI-related visual activation and found a positive relationship between the two in controls but a negative one in DLB. This double dissociation suggests a loss of inhibition in the visual system in DLB, which may predispose individuals to visual dysfunction and visual hallucinations.

Type
Short report
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) licence.
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2016

Dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) is characterised by complex visual hallucinations. Previously, we used the threshold for eliciting phosphenes with occipital transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) as a measure of visual cortical excitability. Reference Taylor, Firbank, Barnett, Pearce, Livingstone and Mosimann1 Although this threshold correlated with the severity and frequency of visual hallucinations in DLB, overall excitability was similar between DLB and controls, suggesting that early visual areas are functionally intact in DLB. This was supported by our finding of unaltered cortical activity in the early visual system (V1, V2/V3), as measured by blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), in DLB. Reference Taylor, Firbank, He, Barnett, Pearce and Livingstone2 BOLD haemodynamic response has been suggested to be related to synaptic activity in terms of input and local processing, Reference Logothetis3 as well as to neuronal spiking Reference Lee, Durand, Gradinaru, Zhang, Goshen and Kim4 arising from sensory stimulation, and thus may represent the activity in both excitatory and inhibitory neuronal populations. In contrast, the neuronal discharge consequent to TMS is likely to be dependent on the relative balance of excitatory and inhibitory activity within the area of stimulation. Therefore a positive relationship between BOLD activity in the early visual system and phosphene threshold may suggest that inhibition outweighs excitation, whereas a negative relationship may indicate the opposite. In this paper we report the interrelationship between fMRI BOLD activity in the early visual system and visual cortical excitability as measured using TMS, using previously published data from patients with DLB and hallucinations and from controls. Reference Taylor, Firbank, Barnett, Pearce, Livingstone and Mosimann1,Reference Taylor, Firbank, He, Barnett, Pearce and Livingstone2

Method

Details of participant selection, neuropsychological and neuropsychiatric assessment, fMRI and TMS testing are described fully elsewhere. In the current analysis, we selected participants who had undergone both fMRI and occipital TMS. Reference Taylor, Firbank, Barnett, Pearce, Livingstone and Mosimann1,Reference Taylor, Firbank, He, Barnett, Pearce and Livingstone2 In brief, 16 participants who met criteria for probable DLB Reference McKeith, Dickson, Lowe, Emre, O'Brien and Feldman5 and who experienced visual hallucinations at least once in the month before TMS/fMRI testing were recruited from a community-dwelling population of patients, along with 19 controls. The study was approved by the local ethics committee. The DLB diagnosis was made independently by two experienced senior clinicians. Reference McKeith, Dickson, Lowe, Emre, O'Brien and Feldman5 Occipital TMS and neuroimaging evaluations were conducted within 2 weeks of each other.

TMS and imaging protocols are described in the online supplementary methods.

Results

The DLB and control groups were similar in terms of age, gender and visual acuity. Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) Reference Fahn and Elton6 motor scores were significantly higher and cognitive test and visuoperceptual task scores significantly lower in the DLB group (online Table DS1).

Phosphenes were elicited in a similar proportion of the control (17/19, 90%) and DLB groups (14/17, 82%; Fisher exact test, P = 0.66). Across groups, phosphene thresholds were similar (control group: median 70.7%, interquartile range (IQR) = 44.0%; DLB group: median 77.2%, IQR = 36.0%, Mann–Whitney U-test 115.5, P = 0.23).

There were no differences in V1 or V2/V3 BOLD magnitude between the control and DLB groups for checkerboard stimulus (V1: control group, mean 0.41 (s.d. = 0.36); DLB group, mean 0.57 (s.d. = 0.30), P = 0.16. V2/V3 control group: mean 0.68 (s.d. = 0.36); DLB group mean 0.71 (s.d. = 0.29), P= 0.83). Similarly, there were no perfusion differences in V1 (ml/100 g/min: control group, mean 33.3 (s.d.= 9.6); DLB group, mean 28.0 (s.d. = 10.5), P= 0.20) or V2/V3 (control group, mean 32.1 (s.d. = 8.4), DLB group, mean 27.0 (s.d. = 7.6), P= 0.15) between groups. However, there were significant positive correlations between the magnitude of the BOLD response (V1 and V2/V3) to the checkerboard stimulus and phosphene thresholds in the control group and a significant negative relationship in the DLB group in both V1 and V2/V3 (V1: control group, ρ= 0.48, P= 0.04; DLB group, ρ= 70.71, P= 0.002. V2/V3: control group, ρ= 0.64, P= 0.003; DLB group, ρ= 70.57, P= 0.02, online Fig. DS1). These relationships remained significant (P<0.05) even with the removal of non-responders (i.e. those participants with a phosphene threshold greater than 100% of the stimulator output) except for phosphene threshold v. V2/V3 in the DLB group. In TMS responders (i.e. those experiencing phosphenes) BOLD activity was significantly lower in the control group in V1 compared with the DLB group (control group, mean 0.40 (s.d. = 0.37); DLB group, mean 0.65 (s.d. = 0.25), P= 0.04).

Differences in the slopes for the control v. DLB group for V1 and V2/V3 were highly significant (Fisher r-to-z transformation: V1, z= 3.78, P<0.001, V2/V3, z= 3.76, P<0.001). BOLD activity was significantly lower in V1 and trending lower in V2/V3 in those in the DLB group who were on cholinesterase inhibitors (V1: mean 0.45 (s.d. = 0.24); V2/V3: mean 0.62 (s.d. = 0.10)) compared with those in the DLB group not taking these agents (V1: mean 0.82 (s.d. = 0.28), P= 0.02; V2/V3: mean 0.88 (s.d. = 0.33), P = 0.10) although phosphene thresholds were similar, as was cerebral perfusion in V1 and V2/V3 between the DLB group on and off cholinesterase inhibitors (phosphene threshold: mean 76.7% (s.d.= 20.2%) v. 78.2% (s.d.= 21.1%), P= 0.90. Perfusion (ml/100g/min): V1, mean 27.1 (s.d.= 9.7) v. 30.4 (s.d.= 13.7), P = 0.62; V2/V3, mean 27.4 (s.d.= 7.3) v. mean 25.8 (s.d. = 9.4), P = 0.73). There was no association between global cognitive function in the DLB group and phosphene threshold, BOLD activity or cerebral perfusion in V1 or V2/V3 (P>0.13).

Discussion

We report a significant positive relationship between phosphene threshold and BOLD activity in early visual areas (V1, V2/V3) in our controls, which suggests that the greater an individual's inherent visual cortical excitability, the smaller the visual cortical activation in response to a simple checkerboard stimulus. This observation implies that individuals with sensitive visual systems need less ‘activation’ to an external stimulus. In contrast, the opposite relationship in DLB infers a breakdown in this dynamic. Assuming similar visual cortical neuronal populations are activated by the checkerboard and TMS, then in the controls a positive relationship between BOLD activity and phosphene threshold implies inhibition must outweigh excitation, whereas in DLB this is reversed. If the BOLD signal represents spiking excitatory neuronal activity, Reference Lee, Durand, Gradinaru, Zhang, Goshen and Kim4 then the lack of difference between groups in mean BOLD activity suggests a loss of inhibition.

There needs to be critical consideration to our interpretations; our knowledge of the underlying neuronal activity of the BOLD signal is incomplete Reference Logothetis3,Reference Logothetis7 and the BOLD effect reflects not just neuronal activity but also haemodynamic coupling, which may be altered with neurodegeneration; ideally cerebrovascular reactivity would be assessed via a CO2 challenge Reference Cantin, Villien, Moreaud, Tropres, Keignart and Chipon8 or similar paradigm. However, we did not observe any differences in occipital perfusion between the control and DLB group, and similarly, there was no relationship between perfusion and either BOLD response or phosphene threshold. Furthermore, cognitive function in DLB was not associated with phosphene threshold or any of the neuroimaging measures. Thus, although not providing a direct measure of cerebrovascular reactivity, these findings suggest that the early visual system in these participants with DLB was similar to controls.

From the TMS perspective, the origin of phosphene perception may not be co-localised to the visual cortex and thus the relative state of the visual cortex may be less crucial to phosphene perception, although reciprocal connections between early visual and association areas are likely to be important. Reference Taylor, Walsh and Eimer9 In addition, cholinergic function has been implicated in modulating visual attention and associated BOLD fMRI responses; Reference Ricciardi, Handjaras, Bernardi, Pietrini and Furey10 our observation of differences in BOLD activity between those patients with DLB taking cholinesterase inhibitors v. those not, supports this and fits into the broader framework of reported cholinergic receptor changes in DLB Reference O'Brien, Colloby, Pakrasi, Perry, Pimlott and Wyper11 as well as changes in occipital metabolism in individuals with DLB with hallucination amelioration treated with donepezil. Reference Satoh, Ishikawa, Meguro, Kasuya, Ishii and Yamaguchi12

Finally, a loss of inhibitory activity in patients might be expected to decrease phosphene threshold relative to the control group, something which was not observed. However, it is notable that for a range of phosphene thresholds, the overall BOLD response in the DLB group was larger than in the control group, significantly so for V1 after excluding TMS non-responders (whose relative level of visual cortical excitability is unclear). Hence, one could speculate that compensatory mechanisms may be operating in patients with DLB that attempt to ‘normalise’ the balance of excitation/ inhibition in an underactive early visual system, in the face of ‘upstream’ pathology in the visual system thus leading to a maintenance of the phosphene threshold within a normative range and possibly improved bottom-up visual processing, but at the cost of increasing the risk of visual hallucination occurrence, as evidenced by our previous work linking phosphene threshold with visual hallucination frequency and severity. Reference Taylor, Firbank, Barnett, Pearce, Livingstone and Mosimann1

In summary, our findings of a double dissociation suggest a marked deviance in visual neuronal processing between patients with DLB who hallucinate v. healthy controls. Whether this represents a specific loss of inhibitory drive in the visual cortex of people with DLB and what role cholinergic function plays remains to be clarified; neurochemical post-mortem studies or novel spectroscopic imaging assessing for example gamma-aminobutyric acid-ergic tone may be helpful in this regard.

Funding

The research was funded by an Intermediate Clinical Fellowship to J.-P.T. (WT088441MA) and also supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Newcastle Biomedical Research Unit based at Newcastle upon Tyne Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust and Newcastle University. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Taylor, JP, Firbank, M, Barnett, N, Pearce, S, Livingstone, A, Mosimann, U, et al. Visual hallucinations in dementia with Lewy bodies: transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Br J Psychiatry 2011; 199: 492500.Google Scholar
2 Taylor, JP, Firbank, MJ, He, J, Barnett, N, Pearce, S, Livingstone, A, et al. Visual cortex in dementia with Lewy bodies: magnetic resonance imaging study. Br J Psychiatry 2012; 200: 491–8.Google Scholar
3 Logothetis, NK. What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature 2008; 453: 869–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4 Lee, JH, Durand, R, Gradinaru, V, Zhang, F, Goshen, I, Kim, DS, et al. Global and local fMRI signals driven by neurons defined optogenetically by type and wiring. Nature 2010; 465: 788–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 McKeith, IG, Dickson, DW, Lowe, J, Emre, M, O'Brien, J T, Feldman, H, et al. Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies. Third report of the DLB consortium. Neurology 2005; 65: 1863–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 Fahn, S, Elton, R, members of the UPDRS Development Committee. Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale. Macmillan Health Care Information, 1987.Google Scholar
7 Logothetis, NK. Bold claims for optogenetics. Nature 2010; 468: E34; discussion E5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Cantin, S, Villien, M, Moreaud, O, Tropres, I, Keignart, S, Chipon, E, et al. Impaired cerebral vasoreactivity to CO2 in Alzheimer's disease using BOLD fMRI. Neuroimage 2011 58: 579–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 Taylor, P, Walsh, V, Eimer, M. The neural signature of phosphene perception. Hum Brain Mapp 2010; 31: 1408–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10 Ricciardi, E, Handjaras, G, Bernardi, G, Pietrini, P, Furey, ML. Cholinergic enhancement reduces functional connectivity and BOLD variability in visual extrastriate cortex during selective attention. Neuropharmacology 2013; 64: 305–13.Google Scholar
11 O'Brien, JT, Colloby, SJ, Pakrasi, S, Perry, EK, Pimlott, SL, Wyper, DJ, et al. Nicotinic [alpha]4[beta]2 receptor binding in dementia with Lewy bodies using 123I-5IA-85380 SPECT demonstrates a link between occipital changes and visual hallucinations. Neuroimage 2008; 40: 1056–63.Google Scholar
12 Satoh, M, Ishikawa, H, Meguro, K, Kasuya, M, Ishii, H, Yamaguchi, S. Improved visual hallucination by donepezil and occipital glucose metabolism in dementia with Lewy bodies: the Osaki-Tajiri project. Eur Neurol 2010; 64: 337–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: PDF

Taylor et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Material

Download Taylor et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 157.3 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.