Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T15:28:52.153Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Effects of E.C.T. on the Drawings of Depressed Patients

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 February 2018

Ralph Hetherington*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Research, Crichton Royal, Dumfries

Extract

A study of the effects of E.C.T. on mental efficiency and retentivity has already been published (Hetherington, 1952). The present paper reports the effects of E.C.T. on drawings made by the same group of patients. Workers such as Prinzhorn (1922), Maclay, Guttmann and Mayer-Gross (1938), Anastasi and Foley (1943), Martin and Weir (1951) and Raven (1951) have been concerned with analysing unrestricted or “free” drawings of widely varying nature. Other workers, such as Burt (1921) and Goodenough (1926) have used restricted drawings usually of “a man” more specifically for the estimation of intelligence. Anastasi and Foley (1941) have reviewed the work done with the drawings of abnormal people, and have drawn attention to the lack of control groups, the rarity of quantitative assessments, and the danger of over-interpretation of the drawings on the part of observers. In order to meet these criticisms, patients' drawings have been compared in the present paper with those of a group of normal untreated people, assessments of the drawings being limited to quantitative measurements of area and position on the page, or to subjective judgments of consistency of style undertaken by several observers working independently.

Type
Part I.—Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1952 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anastasi, A., and Foley, J. P., J. Gen. Psychol., 1941, 25.Google Scholar
Idem., Psychol. Bull., 1942, 39 Google Scholar
Idem, J. Gen. Psychol., 1943, 28.Google Scholar
Burt, C., Mental and Scholastic Tests, 1921.Google Scholar
Goodenough, F. L., The Measurement of Intelligence by Drawing, 1926.Google Scholar
Hetherington, R. R., J. Ment. Sci., 1952, 98.Google Scholar
Liss, E., Amer. J. Orthopsychiat., 1938, 8.Google Scholar
Maclay, W. S., Guttmann, E., and Mayer-Gross, W., Proc. Roy. Soc. Med., 1938, 31.Google Scholar
Martin, A. M., and Weir, A. J., J. Ment. Sci., 1951, 97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfister, H. O., Schweiz. Arch. Neurol. Psychiat., 1934, 34.Google Scholar
Prinzhorn, H., Bildnerei der Geisteskranken, 1922.Google Scholar
Raven, J. C., Controlled Projection for Children, 1951.Google Scholar
Reitman, F., J. Ment. Sci., 1939, 85.Google Scholar
Schube, K., and Cowell, J. Arch. Neurol. Psychiat. (Chicago), 1939, 41.Google Scholar
Stainbrook, E., and Löwenbach, H., J. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 1944, 991.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.